Light stimulus change evokes an activity response in the rat
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] D C Blanchard,et al. Antipredator defensive behaviors in a visible burrow system. , 1989, Journal of comparative psychology.
[2] Fear-potentiated startle using three conditioned stimulus modalities , 1994 .
[3] M. Fanselow. The postshock activity burst , 1982 .
[4] M. Fanselow,et al. Changes in feeding and foraging patterns as an antipredator defensive strategy: a laboratory simulation using aversive stimulation in a closed economy. , 1988, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[5] N. E. Grossen,et al. Species-specific behavior and acquisition of avoidance behavior in rats. , 1972, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[6] F P Valle,et al. Effects of strain, sex, and illumination on open-field behavior of rats. , 1970, The American journal of psychology.
[7] R. C. Honey,et al. Negative priming in associative learning: evidence from a serial-conditioning procedure. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[8] R. B. Lockard. Experimental inhibition of activity of kangaroo rats in the natural habitat by an artificial moon. , 1975 .
[9] I. Whishaw. Light avoidance in normal rats and rats with primary visual system lesions , 1974 .
[10] Michael Davis,et al. Anxiogenic effects of high illumination levels assessed with the acoustic startle response in rats , 1997, Biological Psychiatry.
[11] D. Blanchard,et al. Sex differences in the incidence and sonographic characteristics of antipredator ultrasonic cries in the laboratory rat (Rattus norvegicus). , 1992, Journal of comparative psychology.
[12] S. File,et al. CAN SOCIAL INTERACTION BE USED TO MEASURE ANXIETY? , 1978, British journal of pharmacology.
[13] M. Fanselow,et al. The enhancement and reduction of defensive fighting by naloxone pretreatment , 1982 .
[14] M. Fanselow,et al. Modulation of an activity response with associative and nonassociative fear in the rat: A lighting differential influences the form of defensive behavior evoked after fear conditioning , 2005, Learning & behavior.
[15] Michael Davis,et al. Double Dissociation between the Involvement of the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis and the Central Nucleus of the Amygdala in Startle Increases Produced by Conditioned versus Unconditioned Fear , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[16] W. Timberlake,et al. Stimulus control and function of arm and wall travel by rats on a radial arm floor maze , 1999 .
[17] Michael S. Fanselow,et al. A functional behavioristic approach to aversively motivated behavior: Predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behavior. , 1988 .
[18] D. Williams. Maze exploration in the rat under different levels of illumination. , 1971, Animal behaviour.
[19] M. Fanselow. The adaptive function of conditioned defensive behavior: an ecological approach to Pavlovian stimulus-substitution theory , 1989 .
[20] Lee N. Robins,et al. Psychiatric Disorders in America: The Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study , 1990 .
[21] M. Fanselow,et al. Aversively motivated changes in meal patterns of rats in a closed economy: The effects of shock density , 1993 .
[22] M. Fanselow. Factors governing one-trial contextual conditioning , 1990 .
[23] E. M. Lee,et al. Movement arrest and the hippocampus , 1977 .
[24] S. Mineka,et al. A modern learning theory perspective on the etiology of panic disorder. , 2001, Psychological review.
[25] K. Roth,et al. Stress, behavioral arousal, and open field activity—A reexamination of emotionality in the rat , 1979, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[26] R N Walsh,et al. The Open-Field Test: a critical review. , 1976, Psychological bulletin.