Retention of a Time Pressure Heuristic in a Target Identification Task
暂无分享,去创建一个
David Trafimow | David Keller | Joshua Sandry | Stephen Rice | D. Trafimow | Stephen Rice | David Keller | J. Sandry
[1] Mark W. Scerbo,et al. Automation-induced complacency for monitoring highly reliable systems: the role of task complexity, system experience, and operator trust , 2007 .
[2] Erica L. Wohldmann,et al. Specificity effects in training and transfer of speeded responses. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[3] Jonathan Evans. Hypothetical Thinking: Dual Processes in Reasoning and Judgement , 2007 .
[4] Wim De Neys,et al. Dual Processing in Reasoning , 2006, Psychological science.
[5] Erica L. Wohldmann,et al. Skill training, retention, and transfer: The effects of a concurrent secondary task , 2005, Memory & cognition.
[6] Christopher D. Wickens,et al. Automation Reliability in Unmanned Aerial Vehicle Control: A Reliance-Compliance Model of Automation Dependence in High Workload , 2006, Hum. Factors.
[7] J. G. Hollands,et al. Engineering Psychology and Human Performance , 1984 .
[8] John D. Lee,et al. Trust in Automation: Designing for Appropriate Reliance , 2004, Hum. Factors.
[9] M. Bouton. Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.
[10] A. M. Rich,et al. Automated diagnostic aids: The effects of aid reliability on users' trust and reliance , 2001 .
[11] Jonathan Evans. Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.
[12] Eric-Jan Wagenmakers,et al. The effects of time pressure on chess skill: an investigation into fast and slow processes underlying expert performance , 2007, Psychological research.
[13] A. John Maule,et al. The effects of time pressure on human judgement and decision making , 1997 .
[14] A. Baddeley,et al. Context-dependent memory in two natural environments: on land and underwater. , 1975 .
[15] Arthur F Kramer,et al. Age and automation interact to influence performance of a simulated luggage screening task. , 2006, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.
[16] J. P. Morgan,et al. Design and Analysis: A Researcher's Handbook , 2005, Technometrics.
[17] Raja Parasuraman,et al. Performance Consequences of Automation-Induced 'Complacency' , 1993 .
[18] P M Todd,et al. Précis of Simple heuristics that make us smart , 2000, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[19] S. Sloman. The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. , 1996 .
[20] R. Hogarth. Beyond discrete biases: Functional and dysfunctional aspects of judgmental heuristics. , 1981 .
[21] G Gigerenzer,et al. Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality. , 1996, Psychological review.
[22] Christopher D. Wickens,et al. The benefits of imperfect diagnostic automation: a synthesis of the literature , 2007 .
[23] Christopher D. Wickens,et al. A model for types and levels of human interaction with automation , 2000, IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybern. Part A.
[24] Christopher D. Wickens,et al. On the Independence of Compliance and Reliance: Are Automation False Alarms Worse Than Misses? , 2007, Hum. Factors.
[25] Jonathan Evans,et al. Rationality and reasoning , 1996 .
[26] Christopher D. Wickens,et al. Workload and Reliability of Predictor Displays in Aircraft Traffic Avoidance , 2000 .
[27] Ulrich Hoffrage,et al. Inferences under time pressure: how opportunity costs affect strategy selection. , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[28] Ola Svenson,et al. Time pressure and stress in human judgment and decision making , 1993 .
[29] G. Hockey,et al. Effects of time-pressure on decision-making under uncertainty: changes in affective state and information processing strategy. , 2000, Acta psychologica.
[30] Eric J. Johnson,et al. The adaptive decision maker , 1993 .
[31] Rhona Flin,et al. Decision making under stress : emerging themes and applications , 1998 .
[32] Gary Klein,et al. Sources of Power: How People Make Decisions , 2017 .
[33] P. Fitts. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.
[34] Transfer of learning in choice reactions: contributions of specific and general components of manual responses. , 2009, Acta psychologica.
[35] John W. Payne,et al. The adaptive decision maker: Name index , 1993 .
[36] Raja Parasuraman,et al. Humans and Automation: Use, Misuse, Disuse, Abuse , 1997, Hum. Factors.
[37] Alison M. Bacon,et al. Reasoning under time pressure. A study of causal conditional inference. , 2009, Experimental psychology.
[38] D. Kahneman,et al. Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. , 2002 .
[39] Stephen Rice. Examining Single- and Multiple-Process Theories of Trust in Automation , 2009, The Journal of general psychology.
[40] Mark P. Zanna,et al. Time Pressure and Information Integration in Social Judgment , 1993 .
[41] Ola Svenson,et al. Framing and Time Pressure in Decision Making , 1993 .
[42] R. Ratcliff,et al. Multialternative decision field theory: a dynamic connectionist model of decision making. , 2001, Psychological review.