Title Lexical leverage : category knowledge boosts real-time novel word recognition in 2-year-olds Permalink

Recent research suggests that infants tend to add words to their vocabulary that are semantically related to other known words, though it is not clear why this pattern emerges. In this paper, we explore whether infants leverage their existing vocabulary and semantic knowledge when interpreting novel label–object mappings in real time. We initially identified categorical domains for which individual 24-month-old infants have relatively higher and lower levels of knowledge, irrespective of overall vocabulary size. Next, we taught infants novel words in these higher and lower knowledge domains and then asked if their subsequent realtime recognition of these items varied as a function of their category knowledge. While our participants successfully acquired the novel label–object mappings in our task, there were important differences in the way infants recognized these words in real time. Namely, infants showed more robust recognition of high (vs. low) domain knowledge words. These findings suggest that dense semantic structure facilitates early word learning and real-time novel word recognition.

[1]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  The role of context in young children's comprehension of negation , 2014 .

[2]  Daniel E. Bontempo,et al.  Online learning from input versus offline memory evolution in adult word learning: effects of neighborhood density and phonologically related practice. , 2014, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[3]  D. Swingley,et al.  Processing of lexical stress cues by young children. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[4]  A. Pittman,et al.  Effect of phonotactic probability and neighborhood density on word-learning configuration by preschoolers with typical development and specific language impairment. , 2014, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[5]  Anne Fernald,et al.  Real-time interpretation of novel events across childhood. , 2014, Journal of memory and language.

[6]  Jessica S. Horst,et al.  Goodnight book: sleep consolidation improves word learning via storybooks , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[7]  D. Barr,et al.  Using a voice to put a name to a face: the psycholinguistics of proper name comprehension. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[8]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Toddlers Activate Lexical Semantic Knowledge in the Absence of Visual Referents: Evidence from Auditory Priming. , 2013, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[9]  Erica H Wojcik,et al.  The Ontogeny of Lexical Networks , 2013, Psychological science.

[10]  Kim Plunkett,et al.  What’s in a link: Associative and taxonomic priming effects in the infant lexicon , 2013, Cognition.

[11]  P. Rämä,et al.  Development of lexical–semantic language system: N400 priming effect for spoken words in 18- and 24-month old children , 2013, Brain and Language.

[12]  Anne Fernald,et al.  SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. , 2013, Developmental science.

[13]  A. Fernald,et al.  Fast mapping, slow learning: Disambiguation of novel word–object mappings in relation to vocabulary learning at 18, 24, and 30months , 2013, Cognition.

[14]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Social and Discourse Contributions to the Determination of Reference in Cross-Situational Word Learning , 2013 .

[15]  Pia Knoeferle,et al.  Can Speaker Gaze Modulate Syntactic Structuring and Thematic Role Assignment during Spoken Sentence Comprehension? , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[16]  N. Mani,et al.  Language nonselective lexical access in bilingual toddlers. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[17]  Thomas T. Hills The company that words keep: comparing the statistical structure of child- versus adult-directed language* , 2012, Journal of Child Language.

[18]  D. Swingley,et al.  The Looking‐While‐Listening Procedure , 2012 .

[19]  D. Swingley,et al.  At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[20]  Marta Kutas,et al.  Mass univariate analysis of event-related brain potentials/fields II: Simulation studies. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[21]  David M. Groppe,et al.  Mass univariate analysis of event-related brain potentials/fields I: a critical tutorial review. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[22]  Kim Plunkett,et al.  Phonological priming and cohort effects in toddlers , 2011, Cognition.

[23]  J. Saffran,et al.  Interactions between statistical and semantic information in infant language development. , 2011, Developmental science.

[24]  A. Meyer,et al.  Using the visual world paradigm to study language processing: a review and critical evaluation. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[25]  Jill R. Hoover,et al.  The influence of part-word phonotactic probability/neighborhood density on word learning by preschool children varying in expressive vocabulary. , 2011, Journal of child language.

[26]  Thomas T. Hills,et al.  Small Worlds and Semantic Network Growth in Typical and Late Talkers , 2011, PloS one.

[27]  Chen Yu,et al.  What you learn is what you see: using eye movements to study infant cross-situational word learning. , 2011, Developmental science.

[28]  Thomas T. Hills,et al.  The Associative Structure of Language: Contextual Diversity in Early Word Learning. , 2010, Journal of memory and language.

[29]  Dorthe Bleses,et al.  CLEX: a cross-linguistic lexical norms database*. , 2010, Journal of child language.

[30]  Jill Lany,et al.  From Statistics to Meaning , 2010, Psychological science.

[31]  K. Plunkett,et al.  Lexical–semantic priming effects during infancy , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[32]  J. Werker,et al.  Infants' learning of novel words in a stochastic environment. , 2009, Developmental psychology.

[33]  Thomas T. Hills,et al.  Longitudinal Analysis of Early Semantic Networks , 2009, Psychological science.

[34]  H. Storkel,et al.  Adult and child semantic neighbors of the Kroll and Potter (1984) nonobjects. , 2009, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[35]  H. Storkel,et al.  The effect of semantic set size on word learning by preschool children. , 2009, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[36]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[37]  I. Moen,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of auditory semantic priming in 24-month-olds , 2007, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[38]  Christoph Scheepers,et al.  Priming ditransitive structures in comprehension , 2007, Cognitive Psychology.

[39]  R. Aslin,et al.  Lexical competition in young children’s word learning , 2007, Cognitive Psychology.

[40]  H. Storkel,et al.  Differentiating phonotactic probability and neighborhood density in adult word learning. , 2006, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[41]  J. Halberda,et al.  Is this a dax which I see before me? Use of the logical argument disjunctive syllogism supports word-learning in children and adults , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[42]  V. Marchman,et al.  MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories , 2006 .

[43]  Magnus Lindgren,et al.  Semantic organization of basic-level words in 20-month-olds: An ERP study , 2006, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[44]  J. Elman,et al.  Language input and semantic categories: a relation between cognition and early word learning , 2006, Journal of Child Language.

[45]  Anne Fernald,et al.  Names in frames: infants interpret words in sentence frames faster than words in isolation. , 2006, Developmental science.

[46]  Holly L. Storkel,et al.  Do children acquire dense neighborhoods? An investigation of similarity neighborhoods in lexical acquisition , 2004, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[47]  E. Hoff The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. , 2003, Child development.

[48]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Object name Learning Provides On-the-Job Training for Attention , 2002, Psychological science.

[49]  H. Storkel,et al.  Learning new words: phonotactic probability in language development. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[50]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  The Large-Scale Structure of Semantic Networks: Statistical Analyses and a Model of Semantic Growth , 2001, Cogn. Sci..

[51]  A. Barabasi,et al.  Emergence of Scaling in Random Networks , 1999 .

[52]  John P. Pinto,et al.  Rapid Gains in Speed of Verbal Processing by Infants in the 2nd Year , 1998 .

[53]  K Plunkett,et al.  Rapid word learning by fifteen-month-olds under tightly controlled conditions. , 1998, Child development.

[54]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Categorization and Its Developmental Relation to Early Language. , 1997, Child development.

[55]  Alison Gopnik,et al.  Cross-linguistic Differences in Early Semantic and Cognitive Development. , 1996 .

[56]  C. Mervis,et al.  Early lexical acquisition and the vocabulary spurt: a response to Goldfield & Reznick , 1995, Journal of Child Language.

[57]  D. Poulin-Dubois,et al.  Early lexical development: the contribution of parental labelling and infants' categorization abilities , 1995, Journal of Child Language.

[58]  Alison Gopnik,et al.  Categorization and Naming: Basic-Level Sorting in Eighteen-Month-Olds and Its Relation to Language , 1992 .

[59]  Alison Gopnik,et al.  Do linguistic differences lead to cognitive differences? A cross-linguistic study of semantic and cognitive development , 1990 .

[60]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  The importance of shape in early lexical learning , 1988 .

[61]  E. Markman,et al.  Children's sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic versus thematic relations , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.

[62]  Ann L. Brown,et al.  Conceptual preference for thematic or taxonomic relations: A nonmonotonic age trend from preschool to old age ☆ , 1979 .

[63]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Mutual Exclusivity and Vocabulary Structure , 2012 .

[64]  Anne Fernald,et al.  Individual differences in lexical processing at 18 months predict vocabulary growth in typically developing and late-talking toddlers. , 2012, Child development.

[65]  Prahlad Gupta,et al.  Learning Novel Neighbors: Distributed mappings help children and connectionist models , 2008 .

[66]  Anne Fernald,et al.  Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language comprehension by infants and young children. , 2008 .

[67]  Amy Perfors,et al.  Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[68]  Paul Boersma,et al.  Praat: doing phonetics by computer , 2003 .