Social tuning of automatic racial attitudes: the role of affiliative motivation.

Consistent with the affiliative social tuning hypothesis, this study showed that the desire to get along with another person shifted participants' automatic attitudes toward the ostensible attitudes of that person. In Experiment 1, the automatic racial attitudes of women but not men emulated those of an experimenter displaying race-egalitarian attitudes or attitudes neutral with respect to race. Mediational analysis revealed that the gender difference in social tuning was mediated by liking for the experimenter. In Experiment 2, the likability of the experimenter was manipulated. Individuals who interacted with a likable experimenter exhibited social tuning more so than did those who interacted with a rude experimenter. These findings suggest that affiliative motives may elicit malleability of automatic attitudes independent of manipulations of social group exemplars.

[1]  C. Gilligan In a Different Voice: Psychological Theory and Women’s Development , 2009 .

[2]  Jeffrey R. Huntsinger,et al.  The Interpersonal Basis of Self-Stereotyping , 2005 .

[3]  Curtis D. Hardin,et al.  Social tuning of the self: consequences for the self-evaluations of stereotype targets. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  Elizabeth W. Dunn,et al.  The relationship between parental racial attitudes and children’s implicit prejudice , 2005 .

[5]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Contextual variations in implicit evaluation. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[6]  I. Blair,et al.  The Malleability of Automatic Stereotypes and Prejudice , 2002 .

[7]  A. Greenwald,et al.  On the malleability of automatic attitudes: combating automatic prejudice with images of admired and disliked individuals. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[8]  Curtis D. Hardin,et al.  Social influence effects on automatic racial prejudice. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  A. Lenton,et al.  Imagining stereotypes away: the moderation of implicit stereotypes through mental imagery. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  C. Judd,et al.  Spontaneous prejudice in context: variability in automatically activated attitudes. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  C. Rusbult,et al.  Attitude alignment in close relationships. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[12]  John A Updegraff,et al.  Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. , 2000, Psychological review.

[13]  G. Moskowitz,et al.  Preconsciously Controlling Stereotyping: Implicitly Activated Egalitarian Goals Prevent the Activation of Stereotypes , 2000 .

[14]  Judith A. Hall,et al.  Gender differences in nonverbal communication of emotion , 2000 .

[15]  Z. Kunda,et al.  Reactions to a black professional: motivated inhibition and activation of conflicting stereotypes. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  P. Gollwitzer,et al.  Preconscious Control of Stereotype Activation Through Chronic Egalitarian Goals , 1999 .

[17]  Christina T. Fong,et al.  Automatic Activation of Stereotypes: The Role of Self-Image Threat , 1998 .

[18]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  S. Cross,et al.  Models of the self: self-construals and gender. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[20]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  What do connectionism and social psychology offer each other? , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  S. Dollinger,et al.  Photographic depictions of the self: Gender and age differences in social connectedness , 1993 .

[22]  H. Giles,et al.  Handbook of language and social psychology , 1992 .

[23]  S. Murray You just don't understand: Women and men in conversation , 1992 .

[24]  E. Higgins,et al.  Handbook of motivation and cognition : foundations of social behavior , 1991 .

[25]  E. Higgins,et al.  Social Cognition and Communication , 1989 .

[26]  K. Deaux,et al.  Putting gender into context: An interactive model of gender-related behavior. , 1987 .

[27]  W. Eaton,et al.  Sex differences in human motor activity level. , 1986, Psychological bulletin.

[28]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[29]  Harry T. Reis,et al.  Sex differences in the intimacy of social interaction: Further examination of potential explanations. , 1985 .

[30]  E. Tory Higgins,et al.  Social encoding and subsequent attitudes, impressions, and memory: "Context-driven" and motivational aspects of processing. , 1984 .

[31]  C. McCann,et al.  Self-monitoring in communicative interactions: Social cognitive consequences of goal-directed message modification , 1983 .

[32]  G. Moskowitz Cognitive social psychology : the Princeton Symposium on the Legacy and Future of Social Cognition , 1981 .

[33]  Elizabeth Long The Reproduction of Mothering: Psychoanalysis and the Sociology of Gender , 1980, Telos.

[34]  Nancy J. Chodorow,et al.  The Reproduction of Mothering: Psychoanalysis and the Sociology of Gender , 1978 .

[35]  Jean M. Converse,et al.  THE EFFECTS OF BLACK AND WHITE INTERVIEWERS ON BLACK RESPONSES IN 1968 , 1971 .

[36]  A. Eagly,et al.  Gender and Helping Behavior : A Meta-Analytic Review of the Social Psychological Literature , 2004 .

[37]  M. Brewer,et al.  What are we really priming? Cue-based versus category-based processing of facial stimuli. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[38]  E. Harmon-Jones,et al.  The regulation of explicit and implicit race bias: the role of motivations to respond without prejudice. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[39]  Nalini Ambady,et al.  Effects of situational power on automatic racial prejudice , 2001 .

[40]  Curtis D. Hardin,et al.  A relational approach to cognition: Shared experience and relationship affirmation in social cognition. , 2001 .

[41]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The mind in the middle: A practical guide to priming and automaticity research. , 2000 .

[42]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  Knowledge acquisition, accessibility, and use in person perception and stereotyping: simulation with a recurrent connectionist network. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[43]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  Data analysis in social psychology. , 1998 .

[44]  Curtis D. Hardin,et al.  Shared reality: How social verification makes the subjective objective. , 1996 .

[45]  Jennifer A. Epstein,et al.  Sex differences in lying: How women and men deal with the dilemma of deceit. , 1993 .

[46]  Michael Lewis,et al.  Lying and deception in everyday life , 1993 .

[47]  Romin W. Tafarodi,et al.  Gender and self-esteem. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[48]  Judith A. Hall Nonverbal sex differences: Accuracy of communication and expressive style. , 1990 .

[49]  P. Devine Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. , 1989 .

[50]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[51]  Howard Schuman,et al.  WHITE RESPONDENTS AND RACE-OF-INTERVIEWER EFFECTS , 1975 .

[52]  E. Miller Handbook of Social Psychology , 1946, Mental Health.