Visual target detection in a distracting background relies on neural encoding of both visual targets and background
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. D. Rugg,et al. Modulation of visual event-related potentials by spatial and non-spatial visual selective attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.
[2] S. Hillyard,et al. Selective attention to stimulus location modulates the steady-state visual evoked potential. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[3] S. Kelly,et al. A supramodal accumulation-to-bound signal that determines perceptual decisions in humans , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.
[4] S. T. Buckland,et al. An Introduction to the Bootstrap. , 1994 .
[5] S. Yantis,et al. Visual attention: control, representation, and time course. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.
[6] J. Theeuwes. Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[7] J. Polich. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[8] M. Bradley,et al. Large-scale neural correlates of affective picture processing. , 2002, Psychophysiology.
[9] S. Hillyard,et al. Modulations of sensory-evoked brain potentials indicate changes in perceptual processing during visual-spatial priming. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[10] N. Lavie,et al. Failures to Ignore Entirely Irrelevant Distractors , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.
[11] Juliana Yordanova,et al. On the relation of movement-related potentials to the go/no-go effect on P3 , 2006, Biological Psychology.
[12] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[13] E. Knudsen. Fundamental components of attention. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.
[14] N. Lavie. Attention, Distraction, and Cognitive Control Under Load , 2010 .
[15] E. Miller,et al. Top-Down Versus Bottom-Up Control of Attention in the Prefrontal and Posterior Parietal Cortices , 2007, Science.
[16] S. A. Hillyard,et al. Sustained division of the attentional spotlight , 2003, Nature.
[17] E. Donchin,et al. The influence of stimulus deviance and novelty on the P300 and novelty P3. , 2002, Psychophysiology.
[18] Fang Fang,et al. Selective spatial attention involves two alpha-band components associated with distinct spatiotemporal and functional characteristics , 2019, NeuroImage.
[19] J L Gallant,et al. Sparse coding and decorrelation in primary visual cortex during natural vision. , 2000, Science.
[20] S. Kelly,et al. The classic P300 encodes a build‐to‐threshold decision variable , 2015, The European journal of neuroscience.
[21] S. Hillyard,et al. Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[22] Steven A. Hillyard,et al. Attention Facilitates Multiple Stimulus Features in Parallel in Human Visual Cortex , 2008, Current Biology.
[23] Andreas Keil,et al. Time course of competition for visual processing resources between emotional pictures and foreground task. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.
[24] Fang Fang,et al. Sequential sampling of visual objects during sustained attention , 2017, PLoS biology.
[25] Thomas C. Sprague,et al. Dissociable signatures of visual salience and behavioral relevance across attentional priority maps in human cortex , 2017, bioRxiv.
[26] E. Van der Burg,et al. The role of spatial and nonspatial information in visual selection. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[27] E. Vogel,et al. Human Variation in Overriding Attentional Capture , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[28] C. Eriksen,et al. Decision making and the P300 component of the cortical evoked response , 1974 .
[29] E. Viding,et al. Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[30] P. O. Bishop,et al. Spatial vision. , 1971, Annual review of psychology.
[31] Joseph B. Hopfinger,et al. Interactions between endogenous and exogenous attention on cortical visual processing , 2006, NeuroImage.
[32] M. Carrasco,et al. The eccentricity effect: Target eccentricity affects performance on conjunction searches , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[33] R. Barry,et al. Movement-related potentials in the Go/NoGo task: The P3 reflects both cognitive and motor inhibition , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[34] J. Theeuwes. Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.
[35] S. Yantis. 3 Goal-Directed and Stimulus-Driven Determinants of Attentional Control , 2000 .
[36] Susan L. Travis,et al. Neural Responses to Target Features outside a Search Array Are Enhanced during Conjunction but Not Unique-Feature Search , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[37] E. Donchin,et al. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[38] D. E. Irwin,et al. Our Eyes do Not Always Go Where we Want Them to Go: Capture of the Eyes by New Objects , 1998 .
[39] Li Fei-Fei,et al. Neural mechanisms of rapid natural scene categorization in human visual cortex , 2009, Nature.
[40] A. Kok. Effects of degradation of visual stimuli on components of the event-related potential (ERP) in go/nogo reaction tasks , 1986, Biological Psychology.
[41] P. Perona,et al. Rapid natural scene categorization in the near absence of attention , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[42] Yaoda Xu,et al. Decoding the content of visual short-term memory under distraction in occipital and parietal areas , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.
[43] R. Desimone,et al. Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. , 1985, Science.
[44] Nai Ding,et al. Perceptual integration rapidly activates dorsal visual pathway to guide local processing in early visual areas , 2017, PLoS biology.
[45] E Donchin,et al. A metric for thought: a comparison of P300 latency and reaction time. , 1981, Science.
[46] J. Duncan,et al. Competitive brain activity in visual attention , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[47] N. Lavie. Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[48] M. Kenward,et al. An Introduction to the Bootstrap , 2007 .
[49] M Carrasco,et al. The interaction of objective and subjective organizations in a localization search task , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[50] Søren K. Andersen,et al. Effects of Feature-selective and Spatial Attention at Different Stages of Visual Processing , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[51] J. Serences,et al. Value-based attentional capture influences context-dependent decision-making. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.
[52] J. Polich,et al. P3a, perceptual distinctiveness, and stimulus modality. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[53] J. Serences,et al. Two different mechanisms support selective attention at different phases of training , 2016, PLoS biology.
[54] E. Courchesne,et al. Stimulus novelty, task relevance and the visual evoked potential in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[55] John K. Tsotsos,et al. Direct neurophysiological evidence for spatial suppression surrounding the focus of attention in vision. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[56] D. Jeffreys,et al. Source locations of pattern-specific components of human visual evoked potentials. I. Component of striate cortical origin , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[57] Rolf Verleger,et al. Is P3 a strategic or a tactical component? Relationships of P3 sub-components to response times in oddball tasks with go, no-go and choice responses , 2016, NeuroImage.
[58] Jasna Martinovic,et al. Bottom-Up Biases in Feature-Selective Attention , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[59] A. Wilkins,et al. Photic‐ and Pattern‐induced Seizures: A Review for the Epilepsy Foundation of America Working Group , 2005, Epilepsia.
[60] D. Regan. Some characteristics of average steady-state and transient responses evoked by modulated light. , 1966, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[61] B. C. Motter. Focal attention produces spatially selective processing in visual cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 in the presence of competing stimuli. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.
[62] R. Desimone,et al. High-Frequency, Long-Range Coupling Between Prefrontal and Visual Cortex During Attention , 2009, Science.
[63] John T. Serences,et al. Coexisting representations of sensory and mnemonic information in human visual cortex , 2019, Nature Neuroscience.
[64] S. Hillyard,et al. Electrical Signs of Selective Attention in the Human Brain , 1973, Science.
[65] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Attention selection, distractor suppression and N2pc , 2009, Cortex.
[66] S. Kelly,et al. Internal and External Influences on the Rate of Sensory Evidence Accumulation in the Human Brain , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[67] Carly J. Leonard,et al. Hyperfocusing of Attention on Goal-Related Information in Schizophrenia: Evidence From Electrophysiology , 2017, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[68] F. Kitterle. Psychophysics of lateral tachistoscopic presentation , 1986, Brain and Cognition.
[69] Guillaume A. Rousselet,et al. Robust Correlation Analyses: False Positive and Power Validation Using a New Open Source Matlab Toolbox , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[70] Andrew B. Leber,et al. Coordination of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Attentional Control in Human Cortex , 2005, Psychological science.
[71] S. Andersen,et al. Behavioral performance follows the time course of neural facilitation and suppression during cued shifts of feature-selective attention , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[72] Barak A. Pearlmutter,et al. The VESPA: A method for the rapid estimation of a visual evoked potential , 2006, NeuroImage.
[73] M. Paradiso,et al. Feature-specific effects of selective visual attention , 1995, Vision Research.
[74] Viola S. Störmer,et al. Cross-modal cueing of attention alters appearance and early cortical processing of visual stimuli , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[75] G. McCarthy,et al. Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. , 1977, Science.
[76] G. Boynton,et al. Global feature-based attention for motion and color , 2003, Vision Research.
[77] Edward F. Ester,et al. Sensory Gain Outperforms Efficient Readout Mechanisms in Predicting Attention-Related Improvements in Behavior , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[78] Antigona Martínez,et al. Source analysis of event-related cortical activity during visuo-spatial attention. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.
[79] E. Donchin,et al. Auditory averaged evoked potentials in man during selective binaural listening. , 1970, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[80] Hubert R. Dinse,et al. Improvement and Impairment of Visually Guided Behavior through LTP- and LTD-like Exposure-Based Visual Learning , 2011, Current Biology.
[81] Stefan Treue,et al. Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex , 1999, Nature.
[82] David J. Prime,et al. Inability to suppress salient distractors predicts low visual working memory capacity , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[83] Jeff Miller,et al. Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences. , 1998, Psychophysiology.
[84] E. Donchin,et al. Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[85] S. Hillyard,et al. Allocation of visual attention to spatial locations: Tradeoff functions for event-related brain potentials and detection performance , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[86] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[87] S A Hillyard,et al. Feature-selective attention enhances color signals in early visual areas of the human brain , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[88] Y. Benjamini,et al. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing , 1995 .
[89] A. Keil,et al. Neural Substrate of the Late Positive Potential in Emotional Processing , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.