The Role of Imagery and Emotion in the Translation of Concepts into Product Form

ABSTRACT This paper represents a portion of a comprehensive design cognition study focused on the embodiment of concepts with varying emotional content (i.e. concrete, abstract and emotional) into product form. It is aimed at identifying differences in terms of cognitive processing among these three states during the initial part of the design process and during the creation of a new product by professional designers via the use of design briefs. A mixed-methods approach was used including both qualitative and quantitative approaches, and retrospective protocol analysis was used as a primary source of data analysis. Results showed that imageability and context availability ratings for abstract, concrete and emotion concepts are highly related. Findings are discussed with reference to models of cognition and design.

[1]  Willemien Visser,et al.  Design: One, but in different forms , 2007, ArXiv.

[2]  Dinkar Sharma,et al.  Intrusive Cognitions: An Investigation of the Emotional Stroop Task , 1995 .

[3]  M.W.M. Van den Toorn,et al.  The role of hand drawing in basic design education in the digital age , 2012 .

[4]  J. Russell A circumplex model of affect. , 1980 .

[5]  P. Schwanenflugel,et al.  Context availability and the recall of abstract and concrete words , 1992, Memory & cognition.

[6]  A. Leleu,et al.  Social identity-based motivation modulates attention bias toward negative information: an event-related brain potential study , 2011, Socioaffective neuroscience & psychology.

[7]  Jeanette Altarriba,et al.  The distinctiveness of emotion concepts: a comparison between emotion, abstract, and concrete words. , 2004, The American journal of psychology.

[8]  Lassi A. Liikkanen,et al.  CONTEXTUAL CUEING AND VERBAL STIMULI IN DESIGN IDEA GENERATION , 2006 .

[9]  P. Niedenthal Embodying Emotion , 2007, Science.

[10]  J. Russell Culture and the categorization of emotions. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[11]  Jonathon W. Moses,et al.  Ways of Knowing , 2007 .

[12]  J. Russell,et al.  Multidimensional scaling of emotional facial expressions: Similarity from preschoolers to adults. , 1985 .

[13]  Michael J Cortese,et al.  Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[14]  P. Niedenthal,et al.  Embodiment of emotion concepts. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[15]  Willemien Visser,et al.  The Cognitive Artifacts of Designing , 2006 .

[16]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports as Data , 1984 .

[17]  J. V. Bradley Complete Counterbalancing of Immediate Sequential Effects in a Latin Square Design , 1958 .

[18]  Ö. Akin Psychology of architectural design , 1986 .

[19]  R. W. Stowe,et al.  Context availability and lexical decisions for abstract and concrete words , 1988 .

[20]  D. Howard,et al.  Age of acquisition and imageability ratings for a large set of words, including verbs and function words , 2001, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[21]  B. Kushner Descartes' error. , 1998, Journal of AAPOS : the official publication of the American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus.

[22]  Jami J. Shah,et al.  Evaluation of idea generation methods for conceptual design: Effectiveness metrics and design of experiments , 2000 .

[23]  D. Kieras Beyond pictures and words: Alternative information-processing models for imagery effect in verbal memory , 1978 .

[24]  D. A. Grant The latin square principle in the design and analysis of psychological experiments. , 1948, Psychological bulletin.

[25]  N. Cross Designerly ways of knowing , 2006 .

[26]  G. Vigliocco,et al.  The representation of abstract words: why emotion matters. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[27]  Thomas A. Widiger,et al.  Convergent and discriminant validity of measures of cognitive styles and abilities. , 1980 .

[28]  Masaki Suwa,et al.  What do architects and students perceive in their design sketches? A protocol analysis , 1997 .

[29]  Steve Caplin,et al.  Principles Of Design , 2011 .

[30]  Halime Demirkan,et al.  Understanding the Effects of Cognition in Creative Decision Making: A Creativity Model for Enhancing the Design Studio Process , 2007 .

[31]  Howard R. Moskowitz,et al.  Emotion in Concepts , 2008 .

[32]  Paul Ekman,et al.  Facial Expressions of Emotion: New Findings, New Questions , 1992 .

[33]  Lassi A. Liikkanen,et al.  Inspiring design idea generation: insights from a memory-search perspective , 2010 .

[34]  Dana M. Basnight-Brown,et al.  The representation of emotion vs. emotion-laden words in English and Spanish in the Affective Simon Task , 2011 .

[35]  O. John,et al.  Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[36]  James A. Russell,et al.  Multidimensional scaling of emotional facial expressions: Similarity from preschoolers to adults. , 1985 .

[37]  John S. Gero,et al.  Drawings and the design process , 1998 .

[38]  J. Altarriba,et al.  Concreteness, context availability, and imageability ratings and word associations for abstract, concrete, and emotion words , 1999, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[39]  Kristen A. Lindquist,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.11 No.8 Cognitive-emotional interactions Language as context for the , 2022 .