Evidence for the default network's role in spontaneous cognition.

A set of brain regions known as the default network increases its activity when focus on the external world is relaxed. During such moments, participants change their focus of external attention and engage in spontaneous cognitive processes including remembering the past and imagining the future. However, the functional contributions of the default network to shifts in external attention versus internal mentation have been difficult to disentangle because the two processes are correlated under typical circumstances. To address this issue, the present study manipulated factors that promote spontaneous cognition separately from those that change the scope of external attention. Results revealed that the default network increased its activity when spontaneous cognition was maximized but not when participants increased their attention to unpredictable foveal or peripheral stimuli. To examine the nature of participants' spontaneous thoughts, a second experiment used self-report questionnaires to quantify spontaneous thoughts during extended fixation epochs. Thoughts about one's personal past and future comprised a major focus of spontaneous cognition with considerable variability. Activity correlations between the medial temporal lobe and distributed cortical regions within the default network predicted a small, but significant, portion of the observed variability. Collectively, these results suggest that during passive states, activity within the default network reflects spontaneous, internally directed cognitive processes.

[1]  Steve Majerus,et al.  Modulation of medial prefrontal and inferior parietal cortices when thinking about past, present, and future selves , 2010, Social neuroscience.

[2]  R. Buckner,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Fractionation of the Brain's Default Network , 2010, Neuron.

[3]  William W. Graves,et al.  Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Precuneus shares intrinsic functional architecture in humans and monkeys , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  K. Christoff,et al.  Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[6]  D. Schacter,et al.  On the nature of medial temporal lobe contributions to the constructive simulation of future events , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  A D Redish,et al.  Prediction, sequences and the hippocampus , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[8]  Keith A. Johnson,et al.  Cortical Hubs Revealed by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity: Mapping, Assessment of Stability, and Relation to Alzheimer's Disease , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  E. Pastalkova,et al.  Internally Generated Cell Assembly Sequences in the Rat Hippocampus , 2008, Science.

[11]  Peter Fransson,et al.  The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in the default mode network: Evidence from a partial correlation network analysis , 2008, NeuroImage.

[12]  J. Smallwood,et al.  When attention matters: The curious incident of the wandering mind , 2008, Memory & cognition.

[13]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Distinct cortical anatomy linked to subregions of the medial temporal lobe revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  O. Sporns,et al.  Mapping the Structural Core of Human Cerebral Cortex , 2008, PLoS biology.

[15]  A. Engel,et al.  Prediction of human errors by maladaptive changes in event-related brain networks , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[16]  E. Denkova,et al.  Experiencing past and future personal events: Functional neuroimaging evidence on the neural bases of mental time travel , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[17]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Brain's Default Network , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[18]  Alana T. Wong,et al.  Age-Related Changes in the Episodic Simulation of Future Events , 2008, Psychological science.

[19]  Adam Johnson,et al.  Neural Ensembles in CA3 Transiently Encode Paths Forward of the Animal at a Decision Point , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  G. Buzsáki,et al.  Forward and reverse hippocampal place-cell sequences during ripples , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Prospection: Experiencing the Future , 2007, Science.

[22]  M. Fox,et al.  Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[23]  D. Schacter,et al.  Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[24]  C. Frith,et al.  Comment on "Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought" , 2007, Science.

[25]  M. Bar The proactive brain: using analogies and associations to generate predictions , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  E. Stein,et al.  Cingulate activation increases dynamically with response speed under stimulus unpredictability. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  D. Hassabis,et al.  Deconstructing episodic memory with construction , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  D. Hassabis,et al.  Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[29]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought , 2007, Science.

[30]  K. Szpunar,et al.  Neural substrates of envisioning the future , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  P. Fransson How default is the default mode of brain function? Further evidence from intrinsic BOLD signal fluctuations , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  Benjamin J. Shannon,et al.  Coherent spontaneous activity identifies a hippocampal-parietal memory network. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  The temporal distribution of past and future autobiographical events across the lifespan , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[34]  J. Smallwood,et al.  The restless mind. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[35]  Talma Hendler,et al.  Selective fovea-related deprived activation in retinotopic and high-order visual cortex of human amblyopes , 2006, NeuroImage.

[36]  C. Galletti,et al.  Wide-Field Retinotopy Defines Human Cortical Visual Area V6 , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  R. Buckner,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Correlates of Individual Differences in Memory , 2006, Neuron.

[38]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  M. Linden,et al.  Individual differences in the phenomenology of mental time travel: The effect of vivid visual imagery and emotion regulation strategies , 2006, Consciousness and Cognition.

[40]  David J. Foster,et al.  Reverse replay of behavioural sequences in hippocampal place cells during the awake state , 2006, Nature.

[41]  Jeffrey R. Binder,et al.  Interrupting the “stream of consciousness”: An fMRI investigation , 2006, NeuroImage.

[42]  C. Frith,et al.  Performance-related activity in medial rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) during low-demand tasks. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  D. C. V. Essen A Population-Average, Landmark- and Surface-based (PALS) atlas of human cerebral cortex , 2005, NeuroImage.

[44]  Matthew S. Cain,et al.  Rostral and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex make dissociable contributions during antisaccade error commission , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[45]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  C. Frith,et al.  Involvement of rostral prefrontal cortex in selection between stimulus‐oriented and stimulus‐independent thought , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[47]  Justin M Ream,et al.  Neural Basis of Spontaneous thought Processes , 2004, Cortex.

[48]  M. Linden,et al.  Phenomenal characteristics associated with projecting oneself back into the past and forward into the future: Influence of valence and temporal distance , 2004, Consciousness and Cognition.

[49]  R. Clark,et al.  The medial temporal lobe. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[50]  Rafael Malach,et al.  Functional analysis of the periphery effect in human building related areas , 2004, Human brain mapping.

[51]  M. Greicius,et al.  Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer's disease from healthy aging: Evidence from functional MRI , 2004, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA.

[52]  B. Baars,et al.  Brain, conscious experience and the observing self , 2003, Trends in Neurosciences.

[53]  Masatoshi Itoh,et al.  Thinking of the future and past: the roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes , 2003, NeuroImage.

[54]  J. Binder,et al.  A Parametric Manipulation of Factors Affecting Task-induced Deactivation in Functional Neuroimaging , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[55]  P. Fonlupt,et al.  A neural network elicited by parametric manipulation of the attention load , 2002, Neuroreport.

[56]  J. Kihlstrom,et al.  THE EFFECTS OF EPISODIC MEMORY LOSS ON AN AMNESIC PATIENT ’ S ABILITY TO REMEMBER THE PAST AND IMAGINE THE FUTURE , 2003 .

[57]  Talma Hendler,et al.  Eccentricity Bias as an Organizing Principle for Human High-Order Object Areas , 2002, Neuron.

[58]  R. Malach,et al.  The topography of high-order human object areas , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[59]  S. Vecera,et al.  Psychoanatomical substrates of Bálint's syndrome , 2002, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[60]  C. Atance,et al.  Episodic future thinking , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[61]  Craig E. L. Stark,et al.  When zero is not zero: The problem of ambiguous baseline conditions in fMRI , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[62]  M. Raichle,et al.  Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[63]  Talma Hendler,et al.  Center–periphery organization of human object areas , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[64]  B. Mazoyer,et al.  Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man , 2001, Brain Research Bulletin.

[65]  B. Dainton Précis : Stream of Consciousness , 2004 .

[66]  E. DeYoe,et al.  A physiological correlate of the 'spotlight' of visual attention , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[67]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[68]  Philip McGuire,et al.  Brain activity during stimulus independent thought. , 1996 .

[69]  N C Andreasen,et al.  Remembering the past: two facets of episodic memory explored with positron emission tomography. , 1995, The American journal of psychiatry.

[70]  B. Biswal,et al.  Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo‐planar mri , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[71]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Stimulus-independent thought depends on central executive resources , 1995, Memory & cognition.

[72]  W. M. Cox,et al.  Dimensions of Thought Flow in Everyday Life , 1987 .

[73]  D. Ingvar “Hyperfrontal” distribution of the cerebral grey matter flow in resting wakefulness; on the functional anatomy of the conscious state , 1979, Acta neurologica Scandinavica.

[74]  J. Antrobus,et al.  Studies in the stream of consciousness: Experimental enhancement and suppression of spontaneous cognitive processes , 1966 .

[75]  J. Antrobus,et al.  A Factor-Analytic Study of Daydreaming and Conceptually-Related Cognitive and Personality Variables , 1963, Perceptual and motor skills.

[76]  W. Scoville,et al.  LOSS OF RECENT MEMORY AFTER BILATERAL HIPPOCAMPAL LESIONS , 1957, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[77]  J. de Ajuriaguerra,et al.  Balint's syndrome (psychic paralysis of visual fixation) and its minor forms. , 1954, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[78]  H. Gleitman,et al.  Studies in learning and motivation; equal reinforcements in both end-boxes; followed by shock in one end-box. , 1949, Journal of experimental psychology.

[79]  E. Tolman,et al.  Studies in spatial learning: Orientation and the short-cut. , 1946, Journal of experimental psychology.

[80]  R. Buckner The role of the hippocampus in prediction and imagination. , 2010, Annual review of psychology.

[81]  Archana Venkataraman,et al.  Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: theory, properties, and optimization. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[82]  Alana T. Wong,et al.  Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[83]  J. A. Frost,et al.  Conceptual Processing during the Conscious Resting State: A Functional MRI Study , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[84]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[85]  E. Klinger Structure and functions of fantasy , 1971 .

[86]  J. Singer Daydreaming : an introduction to the experimental study of inner experience , 1966 .

[87]  Jerome L. Singer,et al.  Some Characteristics of Adult Daydreaming , 1961 .