Misanthropy, idealism and attitudes towards animals

Abstract When evaluating the ethical status of an action that harms a nonhuman animal (henceforth animal), one might weigh the benefit to humankind against the cost of the harm done to the animal. To the extent that one does not like humans (is misanthropic), one will not be likely to think that benefits to humans can justify doing harm to animals. We hypothesized that misanthropy would be less strongly related to support for animal rights among idealists (who tend not to do cost-benefit analysis) than among nonidealists. College students (n=154) completed a questionnaire which included questions designed to measure their ethical idealism (ten items), misanthropy (five items), and attitudes towards animal rights and animal research (28 items). Respondents were classified as being idealistic if their score on the idealism scale was greater than the median score. The regression lines for predicting attitudes towards animals from misanthropy differed significantly between idealists and nonidealists. Among nonidealists there was a significant positive relationship between misanthropy and support for animal rights, but among idealists the regression line was flat.

[1]  O. Gingerich Antivivisection and medical science in Victorian society , 1976, Medical History.

[2]  J. Beckstead,et al.  Attitudes toward animal research. , 1988 .

[3]  H. Herzog,et al.  Social Attitudes and Animals , 2001 .

[4]  Robert B. Pittman,et al.  Gender, Sex Role Orientation, and Attitudes toward Animals , 1991 .

[5]  D. Dewsbury,et al.  Early interactions between animal psychologists and animal activists and the founding of the APA Committee on Precautions in Animal Experimentation. , 1990, The American psychologist.

[6]  S. Pious Attitudes Toward the Use of Animals in Psychological Research and Education , 1996 .

[7]  H. Herzog,et al.  The ethical judgment of animal research. , 1992, Ethics & behavior.

[8]  H. G. Osburn,et al.  Coefficient alpha and related internal consistency reliability coefficients. , 2000, Psychological methods.

[9]  R. Ulrich Article Commentary: Animal Rights, Animal Wrongs and the Question of Balance , 1991 .

[10]  H. Herzog,et al.  Ethical ideology and moral persuasion: personal moral philosophy, gender, and judgments of pro- and anti-animal research propaganda. , 1996, Society & animals : social scientific studies of the human experience of other animals.

[11]  S. Plous Attitudes toward the use of animals in psychological research and education: Results from a national survey of psychologists. , 1996 .

[12]  G. Poteat,et al.  Evaluating the morality of animal research: effects of ethical ideology, gender, and purpose. , 1998, Journal of social behavior and personality.

[13]  D. Forsyth,et al.  Ethical Ideology and Judgments of Social Psychological Research: Multidimensional Analysis , 1984 .

[14]  P. Bateson,et al.  Animals in behaviural research , 1988, Animal Behaviour.

[15]  A N Rowan,et al.  Rethinking the morality of animal research. , 1985, The Hastings Center report.

[16]  N. Miller The value of behavioral research on animals. , 1985, The American psychologist.

[17]  H. G. Osburn,et al.  A Method for Maximizing Split-Half Reliability Coefficients , 1977 .

[18]  H. Herzog,et al.  Ethical ideology, animal rights activism, and attitudes toward the treatment of animals. , 1992, Ethics & behavior.

[19]  B. R. Schlenker,et al.  On the ethics of psychological research. , 1977, Journal of experimental social psychology.

[20]  J. W. Driscoll Attitudes Toward Animal Use , 1992 .

[21]  D. Forsyth,et al.  Sexual attitudes and moral values: The importance of idealism and relativism , 1989 .

[22]  D. Forsyth A taxonomy of ethical ideologies. , 1980 .