Integrating Knowledge of Vertically Aligned Large-Scale Spaces

The acquisition and integration of configurational knowledge of spatial layout was studied in a large building complex containing several levels. Twenty-four college students learned two separate routes by walking around the complex; the two were located one above the other, although this was not visibly apparent. Subjects were then given a description that allowed them to integrate their knowledge of the two routes. Straight-line pointing errors and latencies revealed that subjects acquired considerable configurational knowledge about each route and about their relationship, although pointing was slower and less accurate between than within routes. The study demonstrates integration of separately learned spaces in a naturalistic setting, important to theories of environmental learning. It also provides data on learning in vertically aligned spaces and further evidence of the utility of self-report sense of direction as an individual-difference measure.

[1]  Kevin Lynch,et al.  The Image of the City , 1960 .

[2]  A. Siegel,et al.  The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. , 1975, Advances in child development and behavior.

[3]  David Lewis,et al.  We, the Navigators: The Ancient Art of Landfinding in the Pacific , 1975 .

[4]  Arline L. Bronzaft,et al.  Spatial Orientation in a Subway System , 1976 .

[5]  David V. Canter,et al.  Psychology and the built environment , 1976 .

[6]  L. Kozlowski,et al.  Sense of Direction, Spatial Orientation, and Cognitive Maps. , 1977 .

[7]  A. Siegel,et al.  Cognitive maps of a college campus: A new look at freshman orientation , 1979 .

[8]  G. Evans,et al.  Cognitive mapping and architecture. , 1980 .

[9]  G. Evans,et al.  Environmental Learning and Cognitive Mapping , 1981 .

[10]  J. Harvey Cognition, social behavior, and the environment , 1981 .

[11]  T. Gärling,et al.  Acquisition of locational information about reference points during blindfolded and sighted locomotion: effects of a concurrent task and locomotion paths. , 1981, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[12]  I Moar,et al.  Memory for Routes , 1982, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[13]  M M Smyth,et al.  Orientation and spatial representation within multiple frames of reference. , 1982, British journal of psychology.

[14]  K. J. Bryant Personality correlates of sense of direction and geographical orientation. , 1982, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[15]  M. Potegal Spatial abilities : development and physiological foundations , 1982 .

[16]  Barbara Hayes-Roth,et al.  Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[17]  T. Gärling,et al.  Orientation in buildings: Effects of familiarity, visual access, and orientation aids. , 1983 .

[18]  M. Levine,et al.  Spatial problem solving: The integration of independently learned cognitive maps , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[19]  Annabel J. Cohen,et al.  Working Mental Representations of the Environment , 1984 .

[20]  Romedi Passini,et al.  Wayfinding in Architecture , 1984 .

[21]  E. Spelke,et al.  Spatial knowledge in a young blind child , 1984, Cognition.

[22]  Annabel J. Cohen,et al.  Mental Mapping of a Megastructure , 1984 .

[23]  Gary L. Allen,et al.  Expression of Configurational Knowledge of Large-Scale Environments , 1984 .

[24]  Gerard Hanley,et al.  The Placement and Misplacement of You-Are-Here Maps , 1984 .

[25]  Christine A. Klein,et al.  Children's and adults' cognitive maps of very large unfamiliar environments , 1987 .

[26]  Shannon Dawn Moeser Cognitive Mapping in a Complex Building , 1988 .

[27]  D. Holding,et al.  Acquisition of Route Network Knowledge by Males and Females , 1989 .

[28]  Tommy Gärling,et al.  Is elevation encoded in cognitive maps , 1990 .

[29]  J. Peponis,et al.  Finding the Building in Wayfinding , 1990 .