Self-Affirmation Among the Poor

The poor are universally stigmatized. The stigma of poverty includes being perceived as incompetent and feeling shunned and disrespected. It can lead to cognitive distancing, diminish cognitive performance, and cause the poor to forego beneficial programs. In the present research, we examined how self-affirmation can mitigate the stigma of poverty through randomized field experiments involving low-income individuals at an inner-city soup kitchen. Because of low literacy levels, we used an oral rather than written affirmation procedure, in which participants verbally described a personal experience that made them feel successful or proud. Compared with nonaffirmed participants, affirmed individuals exhibited better executive control, higher fluid intelligence, and a greater willingness to avail themselves of benefits programs. The effects were not driven by elevated positive mood, and the same intervention did not affect the performance of wealthy participants. The findings suggest that self-affirmation can improve the cognitive performance and decisions of the poor, and it may have important policy implications.

[1]  D. Amso,et al.  Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  Randall W Engle,et al.  Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[3]  S. Mullainathan,et al.  Scarcity: Why Having Too Little Means So Much , 2013 .

[4]  Geoffrey L. Cohen,et al.  The Psychology of Self‐defense: Self‐Affirmation Theory , 2006 .

[5]  Valerie Purdie-Vaughns,et al.  Deflecting the trajectory and changing the narrative: how self-affirmation affects academic performance and motivation under identity threat. , 2013, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[6]  Geoffrey L. Cohen,et al.  Reducing the Racial Achievement Gap: A Social-Psychological Intervention , 2006, Science.

[7]  Geoffrey L. Cohen,et al.  Self-affirmation theory: An update and appraisal. , 2019, Cognitive dissonance: Reexamining a pivotal theory in psychology (2nd ed.)..

[8]  Valerie Purdie-Vaughns,et al.  Recursive Processes in Self-Affirmation: Intervening to Close the Minority Achievement Gap , 2009, Science.

[9]  Chad E. Forbes,et al.  An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. , 2008, Psychological review.

[10]  P. Horan,et al.  The Social Bases of Welfare Stigma , 1974 .

[11]  Amy J. C. Cuddy,et al.  The BIAS map: behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[12]  S. Spencer,et al.  Stereotype Threat and Women's Math Performance , 1999 .

[13]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[14]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  Preserving Integrity in the Face of Performance Threat , 2012, Psychological science.

[15]  M. Stewart,et al.  “Who Do They Think We Are, Anyway?”: Perceptions of and Responses to Poverty Stigma , 2009, Qualitative health research.

[16]  W. Klein,et al.  Experimental manipulations of self-affirmation: A systematic review , 2006 .

[17]  J. Raven The Raven's Progressive Matrices: Change and Stability over Culture and Time , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[18]  Chris Arney Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth, and Happiness , 2015 .

[19]  Robin H. Rogers-Dillon The dynamics of welfare stigma , 1995 .

[20]  Peter R. Harris,et al.  The Impact of Self‐Affirmation on Health‐Related Cognition and Health Behaviour: Issues and Prospects , 2010 .

[21]  Sendhil Mullainathan,et al.  Behavioral Economics and Marketing in Aid of Decision Making among the Poor , 2006 .

[22]  Harold R. Kerbo The stigma of welfare and a passive poor. , 1976 .

[23]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Technical Report No. 8006. , 1980 .

[24]  C. Steele A threat in the air. How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. , 1997, The American psychologist.

[25]  P. Satz,et al.  Normative data stratified by age and education for the Neuropsychological Screening Battery for Hispanics (NeSBHIS): Initial report , 1996, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[26]  P. Huguet,et al.  Stereotype Threat Strengthens Automatic Recall and Undermines Controlled Processes in Older Adults , 2012, Psychological science.

[27]  J. Shepperd,et al.  Reducing Information Avoidance Through Affirmation , 2012, Psychological science.

[28]  R. J. Kissane What's Need Got to Do with It? Barriers to Use of Nonprofit Social Services , 2003, The Journal of Sociology & Social Welfare.

[29]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to action: Willed and automatic control , 1980 .

[30]  T. Claire,et al.  Extending the Concept of Stereotype Threat to Social Class: The Intellectual Underperformance of Students from Low Socioeconomic Backgrounds , 1998 .

[31]  G. Schwartz,et al.  Consciousness and Self-Regulation , 1976 .

[32]  S. Fiske Envy Up, Scorn Down: How Status Divides Us , 2011 .

[33]  C. Steele The Psychology of Self-Affirmation: Sustaining the Integrity of the Self , 1988 .

[34]  Sendhil Mullainathan,et al.  Poverty Impedes Cognitive Function , 2013, Science.

[35]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.

[36]  E. Shafir The Behavioral Foundations of Public Policy , 2013 .

[37]  J. Currie,et al.  Explaining Recent Declines in Food Stamp Program Participation , 2001 .