Self-Organization and Compositionality in Cognitive Brains: A Neurorobotics Study

The most pressing question about cognitive brains is how they support the compositionality that enables combinatorial manipulations of images, thoughts, and actions. When addressing this problem with synthetic modeling, the conventional idea prevalent in artificial intelligence and cognitive science, generally, is to assume hybrid systems and corresponding neural network models, where higher order cognition is realized by means of symbolic representation and lower sensory-motor processes by analog processing. However, the crucial problem with such approaches is that the symbols represented at higher order cognitive levels cannot be grounded naturally in sensory-motor reality. The former are defined in a discrete space without any metric, and the latter are defined in a continuous space with a physical metric. These, therefore, cannot directly interact with each other, regardless of the interface that is assigned between them. The proposal in the current paper is to reconstruct higher order cognition by means of continuous neurodynamic systems that can elaborate delicate interactions with the sensory-motor level while sharing the same metric space. Our neurorobotics experiments-including language-action associations and the learning of goal-directed actions-show that the compositionality necessary for higher order cognitive tasks can be acquired by means of self-organizing dynamic structures, via interactive learning between the top-down intentional process of acting on the physical world and the bottom-up recognition of perceptual reality. Using robotic simulations, the current paper demonstrates that nonlinear dynamic phenomena, such as bifurcations and the chaotic dynamics induced by unstable fixed points, could play essential roles in realizing higher order functions.

[1]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[2]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Brain mechanisms linking language and action , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[3]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  Jun Tani,et al.  Development of hierarchical structures for actions and motor imagery: a constructivist view from synthetic neuro-robotics study , 2009, Psychological research.

[5]  Karl J. Friston The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[6]  Julia Uddén,et al.  A rostro-caudal gradient of structured sequence processing in the left inferior frontal gyrus , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  Stephen Wiggins,et al.  Global Bifurcations and Chaos , 1988 .

[8]  Stevan Harnad The Symbol Grounding Problem , 1999, ArXiv.

[9]  Stephan K. U. Zibner,et al.  Using Dynamic Field Theory to extend the embodiment stance toward higher cognition , 2013 .

[10]  Jun Tani,et al.  A Neurodynamic Account of Spontaneous Behaviour , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[11]  I. Johnsrude,et al.  Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.

[12]  Stephen Wiggins Global Bifurcations and Chaos: Analytical Methods , 1988 .

[13]  Jun Tani,et al.  How Hierarchical Control Self-organizes in Artificial Adaptive Systems , 2005, Adapt. Behav..

[14]  K. Bach Varieties of Reference , 1994 .

[15]  L. Baum,et al.  Statistical Inference for Probabilistic Functions of Finite State Markov Chains , 1966 .

[16]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The Architecture of Cognitive Control in the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2003, Science.

[17]  Z. Pylyshyn Robot's Dilemma: The Frame Problem in Artificial Intelligence , 1987 .

[18]  J. Fuster Prefrontal Cortex , 2018 .

[19]  K Tanaka,et al.  Neuronal mechanisms of object recognition. , 1993, Science.

[20]  C. Gross Genealogy of the “Grandmother Cell” , 2002, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[21]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  Listening to Action-related Sentences Activates Fronto-parietal Motor Circuits , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  Stephen W. Smoliar Zenon W. Pylyshyn, The Robot's Dilemma: The Frame Problem in Artificial Intelligence , 1988, Artif. Intell..

[23]  J. Fuster,et al.  Prefrontal neurons in networks of executive memory , 2000, Brain Research Bulletin.

[24]  Patricia S. Goldman TOPOGRAPHY OF COGNITION: Parallel Distributed Networks in Primate Association Cortex , 1988 .

[25]  D. Pincus,et al.  How Brains Make Up Their Minds , 2001 .

[26]  Julian M. Pine,et al.  Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. , 2004 .

[27]  Michael A. Arbib,et al.  Perceptual Structures and Distributed Motor Control , 1981 .

[28]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[29]  Jun Tani,et al.  Dynamic and interactive generation of object handling behaviors by a small humanoid robot using a dynamic neural network model , 2006, Neural Networks.

[30]  Jun Tani,et al.  Learning to generate articulated behavior through the bottom-up and the top-down interaction processes , 2003, Neural Networks.

[31]  J A Kelso,et al.  Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. , 1988, Science.

[32]  Ronald J. Williams,et al.  A Learning Algorithm for Continually Running Fully Recurrent Neural Networks , 1989, Neural Computation.

[33]  Symbolic dynamics , 2008, Scholarpedia.

[34]  A. Borst Seeing smells: imaging olfactory learning in bees , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  Jun Tani,et al.  Self-organization of behavioral primitives as multiple attractor dynamics: A robot experiment , 2003, IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybern. Part A.

[36]  J. Tani On the Interactions Between Top-Down Anticipation and Bottom-Up Regression , 2007, Frontiers in neurorobotics.

[37]  Jun Tani,et al.  An Interpretation of the "Self" From the Dynamical Systems Perspective: A Constructivist Approach , 1998 .

[38]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Action outcomes are represented in human inferior frontoparietal cortex. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  J. Duncan,et al.  Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[40]  Ichiro Tsuda,et al.  Memory Dynamics in Asynchronous Neural Networks , 1987 .

[41]  K. Aihara,et al.  Chaotic neural networks , 1990 .

[42]  M. Desmurget,et al.  Movement Intention After Parietal Cortex Stimulation in Humans , 2009, Science.

[43]  Stefano Nolfi,et al.  An Experiment on Behavior Generalization and the Emergence of Linguistic Compositionality in Evolving Robots , 2011, IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development.

[44]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Common cortical and subcortical targets of the dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices in the rhesus monkey: evidence for a distributed neural network subserving spatially guided behavior , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[45]  Jeffrey L. Elman,et al.  Finding Structure in Time , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[46]  Michael I. Jordan Attractor dynamics and parallelism in a connectionist sequential machine , 1990 .

[47]  M. Posner Attention: the mechanisms of consciousness. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[48]  K. Heilman,et al.  Two forms of ideomotor apraxia , 1982, Neurology.

[49]  A. Luria The Working Brain , 1973 .

[50]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Segmenting dynamic human action via statistical structure , 2008, Cognition.

[51]  K. Doya,et al.  Memorizing oscillatory patterns in the analog neuron network , 1989, International 1989 Joint Conference on Neural Networks.

[52]  Young,et al.  Inferring statistical complexity. , 1989, Physical review letters.

[53]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Learning internal representations by error propagation , 1986 .

[54]  Angelo Cangelosi,et al.  From robotic toil to symbolic theft: Grounding transfer from entry-level to higher-level categories1 , 2000, Connect. Sci..

[55]  Teuvo Kohonen,et al.  Self-organized formation of topologically correct feature maps , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[56]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Visual statistical learning in infancy: evidence for a domain general learning mechanism , 2002, Cognition.

[57]  Jun Tani,et al.  Self-organization of distributedly represented multiple behavior schemata in a mirror system: reviews of robot experiments using RNNPB , 2004, Neural Networks.

[58]  Jun Tanji,et al.  Role for supplementary motor area cells in planning several movements ahead , 1994, Nature.

[59]  S. Blakemore,et al.  The learning brain: lessons for education: a précis. , 2005, Developmental science.

[60]  R. Beer Dynamical approaches to cognitive science , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[61]  J. Fodor,et al.  Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis , 1988, Cognition.

[62]  Prof. Dr. H. Liepmann Motorische Aphasie und Apraxie. , 1913 .

[63]  Jun Tani,et al.  Learning Semantic Combinatoriality from the Interaction between Linguistic and Behavioral Processes , 2005, Adapt. Behav..

[64]  Jun Tani,et al.  Emergence of Functional Hierarchy in a Multiple Timescale Neural Network Model: A Humanoid Robot Experiment , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[65]  W. Freeman,et al.  How brains make chaos in order to make sense of the world , 1987, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[66]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[67]  J. Tanji,et al.  Neuronal activity in the primate prefrontal cortex in the process of motor selection based on two behavioral rules. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[68]  Stephen W. Smoliar,et al.  The Robot's dilemma: The frame problem in artificial intelligence: Zenon W. Pylyshyn (Ed.), (Ablex, Norwood, NJ, 1987); xi + 156 pages, $29.50 , 1988 .

[69]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[70]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A Hierarchy of Time-Scales and the Brain , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[71]  M. Goldsmith,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996 .

[72]  M. Brass,et al.  Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[73]  H B Barlow,et al.  Single units and sensation: a neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology? , 1972, Perception.