Temporal Dynamics of Shape Processing Differentiate Contributions of Dorsal and Ventral Visual Pathways

Although shape perception is primarily considered a function of the ventral visual pathway, previous research has shown that both dorsal and ventral pathways represent shape information. Here, we examine whether the shape-selective electrophysiological signals observed in dorsal cortex are a product of the connectivity to ventral cortex or are independently computed. We conducted multiple EEG studies in which we manipulated the input parameters of the stimuli so as to bias processing to either the dorsal or ventral visual pathway. Participants viewed displays of common objects with shape information parametrically degraded across five levels. We measured shape sensitivity by regressing the amplitude of the evoked signal against the degree of stimulus scrambling. Experiment 1, which included grayscale versions of the stimuli, served as a benchmark establishing the temporal pattern of shape processing during typical object perception. These stimuli evoked broad and sustained patterns of shape sensitivity beginning as early as 50 msec after stimulus onset. In Experiments 2 and 3, we calibrated the stimuli such that visual information was delivered primarily through parvocellular inputs, which mainly project to the ventral pathway, or through koniocellular inputs, which mainly project to the dorsal pathway. In the second and third experiments, shape sensitivity was observed, but in distinct spatio-temporal configurations from each other and from that elicited by grayscale inputs. Of particular interest, in the koniocellular condition, shape selectivity emerged earlier than in the parvocellular condition. These findings support the conclusion of distinct dorsal pathway computations of object shape, independent from the ventral pathway.

[1]  R. Turner,et al.  Form and motion coherence activate independent, but not dorsal/ventral segregated, networks in the human brain , 2000, Current Biology.

[2]  Peter Janssen,et al.  Posterior Parietal Cortex Drives Inferotemporal Activations During Three-Dimensional Object Vision , 2016, PLoS biology.

[3]  R. Malach,et al.  Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  Alice Mado Proverbio,et al.  Blue piglets? Electrophysiological evidence for the primacy of shape over color in object recognition. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[5]  Krystel R Huxlin,et al.  New Approaches to Visual Rehabilitation for Cortical Blindness: Outcomes and Putative Mechanisms , 2010, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[6]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[7]  M. Behrmann,et al.  Three‐Dimensional Representations of Objects in Dorsal Cortex are Dissociable from Those in Ventral Cortex , 2017, Cerebral cortex.

[8]  David C. Plaut,et al.  ‘What’ Is Happening in the Dorsal Visual Pathway , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[9]  Katherine Guérard,et al.  Bank of Standardized Stimuli (BOSS) Phase II: 930 New Normative Photos , 2014, PloS one.

[10]  B. Rossion Understanding face perception by means of human electrophysiology , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  T Landis,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for fast visual processing through the human koniocellular pathway when stimuli move. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  K. Dobkins Moving Colors in the Lime Light , 2000, Neuron.

[13]  H. Heinze,et al.  An event-related brain potential study of visual selective attention to conjunctions of color and shape. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[14]  James W. Tanaka,et al.  Activation of Preexisting and Acquired Face Representations: The N250 Event-related Potential as an Index of Face Familiarity , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  B. Wandell,et al.  Development of white matter and reading skills , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[16]  D. Hubel,et al.  Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: anatomy, physiology, and perception. , 1988, Science.

[17]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.

[18]  H. P. Op de Beeck,et al.  Dissociations and Associations between Shape and Category Representations in the Two Visual Pathways , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data , 2010, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[20]  E. Adelson,et al.  Vision with Equiluminant Colour Contrast: 2. A Large-Scale Technique and Observations , 1992, Perception.

[21]  S. Kastner,et al.  Two hierarchically organized neural systems for object information in human visual cortex , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  M. Bar A Cortical Mechanism for Triggering Top-Down Facilitation in Visual Object Recognition , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  Steven J. Luck,et al.  ERPLAB: an open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[24]  S J Luck,et al.  Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  Craig S. Chapman,et al.  Activity patterns in the category‐selective occipitotemporal cortex predict upcoming motor actions , 2013, The European journal of neuroscience.

[26]  G. Mangun Neural mechanisms of visual selective attention. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[27]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  How parallel are the primate visual pathways? , 1993, Annual review of neuroscience.

[28]  Winrich A Freiwald,et al.  Two areas for familiar face recognition in the primate brain , 2017, Science.

[29]  Yaoda Xu The Posterior Parietal Cortex in Adaptive Visual Processing , 2018, Trends in Neurosciences.

[30]  Pedro Faria,et al.  Shape responses in a macaque frontal area connected to posterior parietal cortex , 2018, NeuroImage.

[31]  E. Halgren,et al.  Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Tactile shape discrimination recruits human lateral occipital complex during early perceptual processing , 2010, Human brain mapping.

[33]  M. A. Goodale,et al.  The removal of binocular cues disrupts the calibration of grasping in patients with visual form agnosia , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  V. Casagrande A third parallel visual pathway to primate area V1 , 1994, Trends in Neurosciences.

[35]  Kalanit Grill-Spector,et al.  On object selectivity and the anatomy of the human fusiform gyrus , 2018, NeuroImage.

[36]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[37]  T. Allison,et al.  Electrophysiological Studies of Face Perception in Humans , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  M. Bar,et al.  Magnocellular Projections as the Trigger of Top-Down Facilitation in Recognition , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  B. Renault,et al.  Face and shape repetition effects in humans: a spatio‐temporal ERP study , 1997, Neuroreport.

[40]  L. Jakobson,et al.  A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.

[41]  T. Hendler,et al.  A hierarchical axis of object processing stages in the human visual cortex. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  Jonathan Winawer,et al.  A Major Human White Matter Pathway Between Dorsal and Ventral Visual Cortex. , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[43]  G. Woodman,et al.  Event-related potential studies of attention , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[44]  R. Gregory The Most Expensive Painting in the World , 2007, Perception.

[45]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Identification of early visual evoked potential generators by retinotopic and topographic analyses , 1994 .

[46]  S. Kastner,et al.  The Functional Neuroanatomy of Object Agnosia: A Case Study , 2010, Neuron.

[47]  M. Brodeur,et al.  The Bank of Standardized Stimuli (BOSS), a New Set of 480 Normative Photos of Objects to Be Used as Visual Stimuli in Cognitive Research , 2010, PloS one.

[48]  S. Makeig,et al.  Mining event-related brain dynamics , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[49]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[50]  M. Goodale,et al.  More than blindsight: Case report of a child with extraordinary visual capacity following perinatal bilateral occipital lobe injury , 2017, Neuropsychologia.

[51]  Paul Schrater,et al.  Shape perception reduces activity in human primary visual cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[52]  R. Reid,et al.  The koniocellular pathway in primate vision. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[53]  Marlene Behrmann,et al.  Correction: The large-scale organization of shape processing in the ventral and dorsal pathways. , 2017 .

[54]  M. Eimer ATTENTIONAL MODULATIONS OF EVENT-RELATED BRAIN POTENTIALS SENSITIVE TO FACES , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[55]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  Tool manipulation knowledge is retrieved by way of the ventral visual object processing pathway , 2013, Cortex.

[56]  Roberta L. Klatzky,et al.  Ventral and Dorsal Visual Stream Contributions to the Perception of Object Shape and Object Location , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[57]  Yaoda Xu,et al.  Distinctive Neural Mechanisms Supporting Visual Object Individuation and Identification , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[58]  E. Vogel,et al.  The visual N1 component as an index of a discrimination process. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[59]  Marlene Behrmann,et al.  More than Action: The Dorsal Pathway Contributes to the Perception of 3-D Structure , 2018, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.