The effect of practice on a sustained attention task in cocaine abusers

Habituation enables the organism to attend selectively to novel stimuli by diminishing no-longer necessary responses to repeated stimuli. Because the prefrontal cortex (PFC) has a core role in monitoring attention and behavioral control especially under novelty, neural habituation responses may be modified in drug addiction, a psychopathology that entails PFC abnormalities in both structure and function. Sixteen cocaine abusers and 12 gender-, race-, education-, and intelligence-matched healthy control subjects performed an incentive sustained attention task twice, under novelty and after practice, during functional magnetic resonance imaging. For cocaine abusers practice effects were noted in the PFC (including anterior cingulate cortex/ventromedial rostral PFC, dorsolateral PFC, and medial frontal gyrus) and cerebellum (signal attenuations/decreases: return to baseline); activations in these regions were associated with craving, frequency of use, and length of abstinence. In the control subjects practice effects were instead restricted to posterior brain regions (precuneus and cuneus) (signal amplifications/increases: deactivation away from baseline). Also, only in the cocaine abusers, increased speed of behavioral performance between novelty to practice was associated with a respective attenuation of activation in the thalamus. Overall, we report for the first time a differential pattern of neural responses to repeated presentation of an incentive sustained attention task in cocaine addiction. Our results suggest a disruption in drug addiction of neural habituation to practice that possibly encompasses opponent anterior vs. posterior brain adaptation to the novelty of the experience: overly expeditious for the former but overly protracted for the latter. Overall, cocaine addicted individuals may be predisposed to an increased challenge when required to maintain alertness as a task progresses, not able to optimally utilize a prematurely habituating PFC to compensate with an increased attribution of salience to a desired reward.

[1]  Rita Z. Goldstein,et al.  Role of Dopamine, the Frontal Cortex and Memory Circuits in Drug Addiction: Insight from Imaging Studies , 2002, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[2]  A. Bechara,et al.  Decision-making and addiction (part II): myopia for the future or hypersensitivity to reward? , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  H. Uylings,et al.  Visual attention task performance in Wistar and Lister Hooded rats: response inhibition deficits after medial prefrontal cortex lesions , 1999, Neuroscience.

[4]  D. Tomasi,et al.  Practice-induced changes of brain function during visual attention: a parametric fMRI study at 4 Tesla , 2004, NeuroImage.

[5]  J D Corbit,et al.  An opponent-process theory of motivation. II. Cigarette addiction. , 1973, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[6]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[7]  N. Ramnani The primate cortico-cerebellar system: anatomy and function , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[8]  M. P Milham,et al.  Practice-related effects demonstrate complementary roles of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortices in attentional control , 2003, NeuroImage.

[9]  Michael F. Green,et al.  Training and quality assurance with the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV (SCID-I/P) , 1998, Psychiatry Research.

[10]  M. Raichle,et al.  Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[11]  J S Fowler,et al.  Addiction, a disease of compulsion and drive: involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  W. Schneider,et al.  Neuroimaging studies of practice-related change: fMRI and meta-analytic evidence of a domain-general control network for learning. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[13]  K. Uğurbil,et al.  High contrast and fast three‐dimensional magnetic resonance imaging at high fields , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[14]  B. Pfleiderer,et al.  Altered Habituation in the Auditory Cortex in a Subgroup of Depressed Patients by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2004, Neuropsychobiology.

[15]  S. Petersen,et al.  The effects of practice on the functional anatomy of task performance. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  N. Petry Discounting of money, health, and freedom in substance abusers and controls. , 2003, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[17]  A. Alterman,et al.  Reliability and validity of the Cocaine Selective Severity Assessment. , 1998, Addictive behaviors.

[18]  G. Fein,et al.  Cocaine abusers have reduced auditory P50 amplitude and suppression compared to both normal controls and alcoholics , 1996, Biological Psychiatry.

[19]  E. Damianopoulos,et al.  Cocaine-conditioned behavioral effects: a role for habituation processes , 2003, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[20]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning , 1995, Nature.

[21]  M. Kimura,et al.  Complementary Process to Response Bias in the Centromedian Nucleus of the Thalamus , 2005, Science.

[22]  H Garavan,et al.  A midline dissociation between error-processing and response-conflict monitoring , 2003, NeuroImage.

[24]  Murray B. Stein,et al.  Habituation of attentional networks during emotion processing , 2002, Neuroreport.

[25]  Dimitris Samaras,et al.  Is decreased prefrontal cortical sensitivity to monetary reward associated with impaired motivation and self-control in cocaine addiction? , 2007, The American journal of psychiatry.

[26]  Beatriz Rico,et al.  The Primate Thalamus Is a Key Target for Brain Dopamine , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  T. Robbins,et al.  Reflection Impulsivity in Current and Former Substance Users , 2006, Biological Psychiatry.

[28]  L. Parsons,et al.  Opponent Process Model and Psychostimulant Addiction , 1997, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[29]  J. Stevens,et al.  Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences , 1993 .

[30]  Nicole Wenderoth,et al.  Changes in Brain Activation during the Acquisition of a Multifrequency Bimanual Coordination Task: From the Cognitive Stage to Advanced Levels of Automaticity , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Linda Chang,et al.  k-Space based summary motion detection for functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2003, NeuroImage.

[32]  P. Holcomb,et al.  Frontal and Parietal Components of a Cerebral Network Mediating Voluntary Attention to Novel Events , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  S. Tiffany,et al.  The development of a cocaine craving questionnaire. , 1993, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[34]  F. Bloom,et al.  Cellular and molecular mechanisms of drug dependence. , 1988, Science.

[35]  E. Miller,et al.  Neural Activity in the Primate Prefrontal Cortex during Associative Learning , 1998, Neuron.

[36]  Philip S. Wang,et al.  Unmet need for services and interventions among adolescents with mental disorders. , 2007, The American journal of psychiatry.

[37]  Hugh Garavan,et al.  Practice‐related functional activation changes in a working memory task , 2000, Microscopy research and technique.

[38]  A. Kelly,et al.  Human functional neuroimaging of brain changes associated with practice. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  A functional MRI study of the influence of practice on component processes of working memory , 2004, NeuroImage.

[41]  Yu-Shin Ding,et al.  Behavioral / Systems / Cognitive Activation of Orbital and Medial Prefrontal Cortex by Methylphenidate in Cocaine-Addicted Subjects But Not in Controls : Relevance to Addiction , 2005 .

[42]  D. Metzger,et al.  The Fifth Edition of the Addiction Severity Index. , 1992, Journal of substance abuse treatment.

[43]  J. Stevens Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences , 1986 .

[44]  R. Gonzalez Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences , 2003 .

[45]  A. Cavanna,et al.  The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[46]  Rita Z. Goldstein,et al.  Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. , 2002, The American journal of psychiatry.

[47]  S. H. Ahmed,et al.  Amphetamine-induced conditioned activity does not result from a failure of rats to habituate to novelty , 1996, Psychopharmacology.

[48]  J. Snel,et al.  Impaired executive function in male MDMA (“ecstasy”) users , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[49]  John E. Desmond,et al.  Load- and practice-dependent increases in cerebro-cerebellar activation in verbal working memory: an fMRI study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[50]  Israel Liberzon,et al.  Habituation of Rostral Anterior Cingulate Cortex to Repeated Emotionally Salient Pictures , 2003, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[51]  Gregor Thut,et al.  Activation of the human brain by monetary reward , 1997, Neuroreport.

[52]  P. Kalivas,et al.  Glutamate Transmission in the Nucleus Accumbens Mediates Relapse in Cocaine Addiction , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  N. Volkow,et al.  The orbitofrontal cortex in drug addiction , 2006 .