Foundations of cooperation in young children

Observations and experiments show that human adults preferentially share resources with close relations, with people who have shared with them (reciprocity), and with people who have shared with others (indirect reciprocity). These tendencies are consistent with evolutionary theory but could also reflect the shaping effects of experience or instruction in complex, cooperative, and competitive societies. Here, we report evidence for these three tendencies in 3.5-year-old children, despite their limited experience with complex cooperative networks. Three pillars of mature cooperative behavior therefore appear to have roots extending deep into human development.

[1]  John Q. Patton Meat sharing for coalitional support , 2005 .

[2]  B. Greiner,et al.  Indirect Reciprocity in Cyclical Networks - An Experimental Study - , 2005 .

[3]  Dale F. Hay,et al.  Sharing in the second year of life. , 1976 .

[4]  D. Barash The Biology of Moral Systems, Richard D. Alexander. Aldine, Hawthorne, New York (1987), xx, +301. Price $34.95 hardback, $16.95 paperback , 1987 .

[5]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees. , 2006, Child development.

[6]  D. Fudenberg,et al.  Evolutionary cycles of cooperation and defection. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  George C. Williams,et al.  Adaptation and Natural Selection , 2018 .

[8]  Diane Jones,et al.  Friendship as a facilitator of emotional responsiveness and prosocial interventions among young children. , 1992 .

[9]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Altruistic Helping in Human Infants and Young Chimpanzees , 2006, Science.

[10]  M. Nowak,et al.  Evolution of indirect reciprocity , 2005, Nature.

[11]  P. Richerson,et al.  The evolution of indirect reciprocity , 1989 .

[12]  Dale F. Hay,et al.  Cooperative interactions and sharing between very young children and their parents. , 1979 .

[13]  F. D. de Waal,et al.  Tolerance for inequity may increase with social closeness in chimpanzees , 2005, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[14]  D. Hay,et al.  Prosocial development. , 1994, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[15]  M. Harris RECIPROCITY AND GENEROSITY: SOME DETERMINANTS OF SHARING IN CHILDREN , 1970 .

[16]  V. Braithwaite,et al.  The Long-Term Benefits of Human Generosity in Indirect Reciprocity , 2002, Current Biology.

[17]  E. Staub,et al.  Need for approval, children's sharing behavior, and reciprocity in sharing. , 1970 .

[18]  Sharing in the Second Year of Life. , 1976 .

[19]  C. Darwin The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex: INDEX , 1871 .

[20]  Marc H. Bornstein,et al.  Social, emotional, and personality development , 1988 .

[21]  M. Milinski,et al.  Cooperation through image scoring in humans. , 2000, Science.

[22]  M. Levitt,et al.  Reciprocity of Exchange in Toddler Sharing Behavior. , 1985 .

[23]  W. Hamilton The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I. , 1964, Journal of theoretical biology.

[24]  U. Fischbacher,et al.  Social norms and human cooperation , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  Geetha B. Ramani,et al.  Becoming a social partner with peers: cooperation and social understanding in one- and two-year-olds. , 2006, Child development.

[26]  R. Trivers The Evolution of Reciprocal Altruism , 1971, The Quarterly Review of Biology.

[27]  Michael Gurven,et al.  The Evolution of Contingent Cooperation , 2006, Current Anthropology.

[28]  Jay W. Jackson,et al.  Realistic group conflict theory: A review and evaluation of the theoretical and empirical literature. , 1993 .