Conditioned Superstition: Desire for Control and Consumer Brand Preferences

There are many opportunities in everyday life to associate consumer products with success or failure. For example, when a basketball fan drinks a particular brand of soda while watching her favorite team win a game, she may perceive that this consumption facilitated the victory. Subsequently, the fan may continue to purchase and consume this same item during future games, in an attempt to help the team. This behavior is known as "conditioned superstition." Data from five experiments indicate that preference for lucky products (i.e., those associated with positive outcomes) increases with higher levels of desire for control combined with lower levels of perceived ability to control outcomes (e.g., low generalized self-efficacy). People who express a preference for these lucky products form an illusion of control over future outcomes, so that they perceive superstitious behavior to be an effective strategy to achieve the desired result.

[1]  Claude M. Steele,et al.  DISSONANCE PROCESSES AS SELF-AFFIRMATION , 1983 .

[2]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective on mental health. , 1988, Psychological bulletin.

[3]  G. Fitzsimons Death to Dichotomizing , 2008 .

[4]  C. Steele The Psychology of Self-Affirmation: Sustaining the Integrity of the Self , 1988 .

[5]  A. Bandura Behavior theory and the models of man. , 1974 .

[6]  B. Skinner 'Superstition' in the pigeon. 1948. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[7]  D. Feltz,et al.  Current directions in self-efficacy research in sport , 2011 .

[8]  S. Vyse Believing in Magic: The Psychology of Superstition , 1997 .

[9]  P. Bouts,et al.  Drawing Familiar or Unfamiliar Cards: Stimulus Familiarity, Chance Orientation, and the Illusion of Control , 1992 .

[10]  Lauren G. Block,et al.  Nonconscious effects of peculiar beliefs on consumer psychology and choice , 2011 .

[11]  C M Steele,et al.  Self-image resilience and dissonance: the role of affirmational resources. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[12]  D. Wegner,et al.  Apparent mental causation. Sources of the experience of will. , 1999, The American psychologist.

[13]  Miguel A. Vadillo,et al.  Illusion of Control , 2013, Experimental psychology.

[14]  D. Wegner,et al.  Apparent mental causation , 1999 .

[15]  Chris Janiszewski,et al.  Assessing the Role of Contingency Awareness in Attitudinal Conditioning with Implications for Advertising Research , 1989 .

[16]  H. Cooper,et al.  The desirability of control , 1979 .

[17]  R. Smith,et al.  PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Effects of Coping Skills Training on Generalized Self-Efficacy and Locus of Control , 2004 .

[18]  Lisa G. Aspinwall,et al.  Self-Affirmation Reduces Biased Processing of Health-Risk Information , 1998 .

[19]  J. Weisz,et al.  Changing the world and changing the self: A two-process model of perceived control. , 1982 .

[20]  D. Wegner,et al.  Everyday magical powers: the role of apparent mental causation in the overestimation of personal influence. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  J. Freedman,et al.  The Belief in Good Luck Scale , 1997 .

[22]  Jennifer J. Argo,et al.  Consumer Contamination: How Consumers React to Products Touched by Others , 2006 .

[23]  G. Jahoda The psychology of superstition , 1969 .

[24]  Yuwei Jiang,et al.  The Unique Consequences of Feeling Lucky: Implications for Consumer Behavior , 2009 .

[25]  J. Fitness,et al.  Coping with uncertainty: superstitious strategies and secondary control , 2004 .

[26]  Adam D. Galinsky,et al.  Lacking Control Increases Illusory Pattern Perception , 2008, Science.

[27]  J. Rudski The illusion of control, superstitious belief, and optimism , 2004 .

[28]  S. West,et al.  Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. , 1994 .

[29]  A. Bandura Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. , 1977, Psychology Review.

[30]  J. Tobacyk,et al.  Superstition and Self-Efficacy , 1991 .

[31]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models , 2008, Behavior research methods.

[32]  Chris Janiszewski,et al.  Evaluative Conditioning Procedures and the Resilience of Conditioned Brand Attitudes , 2010 .

[33]  John G. Lynch,et al.  Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and Truths about Mediation Analysis , 2010 .

[34]  David R. Shanks,et al.  Distinguishing Associative and Probabilistic Contrast Theories of Human Contingency Judgment , 1996 .

[35]  Paul A. M. Van Lange,et al.  Superstition as a psychological placebo in top sport , 2006 .

[36]  Paul A. M. Van Lange,et al.  The Psychological Benefits of Superstitious Rituals in Top Sport , 2005 .

[37]  T. Judge,et al.  Are measures of self-esteem, neuroticism, locus of control, and generalized self-efficacy indicators of a common core construct? , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[38]  Lauren G. Block,et al.  Conscious and Nonconscious Components of Superstitious Beliefs in Judgment and Decision Making , 2008 .

[39]  E. Langer,et al.  The effects of choice and enhanced personal responsibility for the aged: a field experiment in an institutional setting. , 1976, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[40]  Giora Keinan,et al.  The Effects of Stress and Desire for Control on Superstitious Behavior , 2002 .

[41]  T. Mussweiler,et al.  Keep Your Fingers Crossed! , 2010, Psychological science.

[42]  Lauren G. Block,et al.  The effect of superstitious beliefs on performance expectations , 2009 .

[43]  Jonathan Guryan,et al.  Gambling at Lucky Stores: Empirical Evidence from State Lottery Sales , 2008 .

[44]  Geoffrey L. Cohen,et al.  Accepting Threatening Information: Self–Affirmation and the Reduction of Defensive Biases , 2002 .

[45]  Peter Carruthers,et al.  The illusion of conscious will , 2007, Synthese.

[46]  Jerry M. Burger,et al.  Desire for control and the illusion of control: The effects of familiarity and sequence of outcomes. , 1986 .

[47]  Jennifer J. Argo,et al.  Positive Consumer Contagion: Responses to Attractive Others in a Retail Context , 2008 .

[48]  J. Houwer,et al.  Evaluative Conditioning , 2012 .

[49]  R. Rescorla A theory of pavlovian conditioning: The effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement , 1972 .

[50]  B. J. Zebb,et al.  Superstitiousness and perceived anxiety control as predictors of psychological distress. , 2003, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[51]  B. Skinner Superstition in the pigeon. , 1948, Journal of experimental psychology.

[52]  J. Rudski,et al.  Malinowski Goes to College: Factors Influencing Students' Use of Ritual and Superstition , 2007, The Journal of general psychology.