The Effect of Visual Uncertainty on Implicit Motor Adaptation
暂无分享,去创建一个
Richard B. Ivry | Hyosub E. Kim | Darius E. Parvin | Guy Avraham | Zixuan Wang | Jonathan S. Tsay | R. Ivry | Guy Avraham | Zixuan Wang | J. Tsay
[1] Konrad P. Körding,et al. Uncertainty of Feedback and State Estimation Determines the Speed of Motor Adaptation , 2009, Front. Comput. Neurosci..
[2] M. D. Ernst. Permutation Methods: A Basis for Exact Inference , 2004 .
[3] Richard B. Ivry,et al. Flexible Cognitive Strategies during Motor Learning , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[4] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[5] Jean-Jacques Orban de Xivry,et al. Internal model recalibration does not deteriorate with age while motor adaptation does , 2019, Neurobiology of Aging.
[6] M. Ernst,et al. The statistical determinants of adaptation rate in human reaching. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[7] M. Schervish,et al. Bayes Factors: What They are and What They are Not , 1999 .
[8] G. M. Redding,et al. Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[9] D. Nozaki,et al. Simultaneous Processing of Information on Multiple Errors in Visuomotor Learning , 2013, PloS one.
[10] B Wallace,et al. Calibration and Alignment are Separable: Evidence From Prism Adaptation , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.
[11] Michael S Landy,et al. Combining Priors and Noisy Visual Cues in a Rapid Pointing Task , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[12] Konrad Paul Kording,et al. Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning , 2004, Nature.
[13] Darius E. Parvin,et al. Credit Assignment in a Motor Decision Making Task Is Influenced by Agency and Not Sensory Prediction Errors , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[14] Michael S Landy,et al. Statistical decision theory and the selection of rapid, goal-directed movements. , 2003, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[15] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[16] M. Sigman,et al. Statistical inference of body representation in the macaque brain , 2019, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[17] Reza Moazzezi,et al. Invariant errors reveal limitations in motor correction rather than constraints on error sensitivity , 2017, Communications Biology.
[18] Daichi Nozaki,et al. Divisively Normalized Integration of Multisensory Error Information Develops Motor Memories Specific to Vision and Proprioception , 2019, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[19] Krista M Bond,et al. Flexible explicit but rigid implicit learning in a visuomotor adaptation task. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.
[20] R. Ivry,et al. Delayed feedback during sensorimotor learning selectively disrupts adaptation but not strategy use. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.
[21] R Core Team,et al. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .
[22] Hyosub E. Kim,et al. The Psychology of Reaching: Action Selection, Movement Implementation, and Sensorimotor Learning. , 2020, Annual review of psychology.
[23] Konrad Paul Kording,et al. Estimating the sources of motor errors for adaptation and generalization , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.
[24] Philip N. Sabes,et al. Multisensory Integration during Motor Planning , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[25] J. Krakauer,et al. An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[26] Jordan A Taylor,et al. Explicit and Implicit Processes Constitute the Fast and Slow Processes of Sensorimotor Learning , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[27] Richard B Ivry,et al. Re-exposure to a sensorimotor perturbation produces opposite effects on explicit and implicit learning processes , 2020 .
[28] D. Wolpert,et al. When Feeling Is More Important Than Seeing in Sensorimotor Adaptation , 2002, Current Biology.
[29] Albert Jin Chung,et al. Perception of Body Ownership Is Driven by Bayesian Sensory Inference , 2015, PloS one.
[30] Guy Avraham,et al. Environmental Consistency Modulation of Error Sensitivity During Motor Adaptation is Explicitly Controlled , 2019, bioRxiv.
[31] Konrad Paul Kording,et al. Relevance of error: what drives motor adaptation? , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[32] Ulrik R. Beierholm,et al. Causal inference in perception , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[33] Scott E. Bevans,et al. Effect of visual error size on saccade adaptation in monkey. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[34] Matthew J. Crossley,et al. Credit assignment in movement-dependent reinforcement learning , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[35] M. Ernst,et al. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.
[36] S. Kitazawa,et al. Effects of delayed visual information on the rate and amount of prism adaptation in the human , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[37] David M. Huberdeau,et al. The Influence of Movement Preparation Time on the Expression of Visuomotor Learning and Savings , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[38] Konrad P. Körding,et al. Estimating the Relevance of World Disturbances to Explain Savings, Interference and Long-Term Motor Adaptation Effects , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[39] Mathias Hegele,et al. Implicit and explicit components of dual adaptation to visuomotor rotations , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.
[40] Benjamin Wallace,et al. Generalization of prism adaptation. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[41] Richard B. Ivry,et al. Characteristics of Implicit Sensorimotor Adaptation Revealed by Task-irrelevant Clamped Feedback , 2017, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[42] O. Donchin,et al. Awareness of Sensorimotor Adaptation to Visual Rotations of Different Size , 2015, PloS one.
[43] Robert J. van Beers,et al. How Does Our Motor System Determine Its Learning Rate? , 2012, PloS one.
[44] Philip N. Sabes,et al. Modeling Sensorimotor Learning with Linear Dynamical Systems , 2006, Neural Computation.
[45] Richard B. Ivry,et al. Continuous Reports of Sensed Hand Position During Sensorimotor Adaptation , 2020, bioRxiv.
[46] Kurt A. Thoroughman,et al. Trial-by-trial transformation of error into sensorimotor adaptation changes with environmental dynamics. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[47] Daniël Lakens,et al. Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs , 2013, Front. Psychol..
[48] Mollie K. Marko,et al. Sensitivity to prediction error in reach adaptation. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.
[49] Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al. Generalization and Multirate Models of Motor Adaptation , 2012, Neural Computation.
[50] R. Held,et al. Adaptation to displaced and delayed visual feedback from the hand. , 1966 .
[51] Shigeru Kitazawa,et al. Prism adaptation with delayed visual error signals in the monkey , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[52] R. Shadmehr,et al. A Shared Resource between Declarative Memory and Motor Memory , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[53] R. Shadmehr,et al. Modulation of error-sensitivity during a prism adaptation task in people with cerebellar degeneration. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.
[54] J. Krakauer,et al. Explicit and Implicit Contributions to Learning in a Sensorimotor Adaptation Task , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[55] Konrad Paul Kording,et al. Causal Inference in Multisensory Perception , 2007, PloS one.
[56] Reza Shadmehr,et al. Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives , 2000, Nature.
[57] Stephen E. Fienberg,et al. Testing Statistical Hypotheses , 2005 .
[58] J. Krakauer,et al. Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.