Assessing recent and remote associative olfactory memory in rats using the social transmission of food preference paradigm

Rats have the ability to learn about potential food sources by sampling their odors on the breath of conspecifics. Although this ethologically based social behavior has been transposed to the laboratory to probe nonspatial associative olfactory memory, only a few studies have taken full advantage of its unique features to examine the organization of recently and remotely acquired information. We provide a set of standardized procedures and technical refinements that are particularly useful in achieving this goal while minimizing confounding factors. These procedures, built upon a three-stage protocol (odor exposure, social interaction and preference test), are designed to optimize performance across variable retention delays, thus enabling the reliable assessment of recent and remote memory, and underlying processes, including encoding, consolidation, retrieval and forgetting. The different variants of the social transmission of food preference paradigm, which take a few days to several weeks to perform, make it an attractive and versatile tool that can be coupled to many applications in CNS research. The paradigm can be easily implemented in a typical rodent facility by personnel with standard animal behavioral expertise.

[1]  C. Gomez-Sanchez,et al.  The effect of chronic food and water restriction on open-field behaviour and serum corticosterone levels in rats , 2000, Laboratory animals.

[2]  P Alvarez,et al.  Differential effects of damage within the hippocampal region on memory for a natural, nonspatial Odor-Odor Association. , 2001, Learning & memory.

[3]  T. Roper,et al.  Social transmission of food-preferences in adult rats , 1983, Animal Behaviour.

[4]  R. Deacon Housing, husbandry and handling of rodents for behavioral experiments , 2006, Nature Protocols.

[5]  N. Grunberg,et al.  The Importance of Reporting Housing and Husbandry in Rat Research , 2011, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[6]  M. Sarter,et al.  Bidirectional interactions between circadian entrainment and cognitive performance. , 2012, Learning & memory.

[7]  Kazumasa Z. Tanaka,et al.  Systems consolidation and the content of memory , 2013, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[8]  B. Galef,et al.  Mother's milk and adult presence: two factors determining initial dietary selection by weanling rats. , 1972, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[9]  K. Luan Phan,et al.  The contextual brain: implications for fear conditioning, extinction and psychopathology , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[10]  David A. Levitsky,et al.  Social transmission of food preferences in adult hooded rats (Rattus norvegicus) , 1984 .

[11]  J. Popic,et al.  Changes of behavioral parameters during long-term food restriction in middle-aged Wistar rats , 2010, Physiology & Behavior.

[12]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Dynamics of Hippocampal and Cortical Activation during Consolidation of a Nonspatial Memory , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  Y. Dudai The restless engram: consolidations never end. , 2012, Annual review of neuroscience.

[14]  Social transmission of information about multiflavored foods , 1992 .

[15]  Larry R Squire,et al.  Anterograde Amnesia and Temporally Graded Retrograde Amnesia for a Nonspatial Memory Task after Lesions of Hippocampus and Subiculum , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  R. Segal,et al.  Journal of Visualized Experiments www.jove.com Video Article Olfactory Behavioral Testing in the Adult Mouse , 2022 .

[17]  Joost X. Maier,et al.  Stability and flexibility of the message carried by semiochemical stimuli, as revealed by devaluation of carbon disulfide followed by social transmission of food preference. , 2014, Behavioral neuroscience.

[18]  L. Toth,et al.  Food and water restriction protocols: physiological and behavioral considerations. , 2000, Contemporary topics in laboratory animal science.

[19]  H. Schiöth,et al.  The relative impact of chronic food restriction and acute food deprivation on plasma hormone levels and hypothalamic neuropeptide expression , 2008, Peptides.

[20]  S. Tufik,et al.  Influence of long-term food restriction on sleep pattern in male rats , 2005, Brain Research.

[21]  R. V. Ruiven,et al.  The influence of transportation stress on selected nutritional parameters to establish the necessary minimum period for adaptation in rat feeding studies , 1998, Laboratory animals.

[22]  R. Neve,et al.  Hippocampal overexpression of mutant creb blocks long-term, but not short-term memory for a socially transmitted food preference. , 2005, Learning & memory.

[23]  Monya Baker,et al.  The ARRIVE guidelines 2019: updated guidelines for reporting animal research , 2019, bioRxiv.

[24]  B. Galef,et al.  Transfer of information concerning distant foods: A laboratory investigation of the ‘information-centre’ hypothesis , 1983, Animal Behaviour.

[25]  Fabio Morellini,et al.  Spatial memory tasks in rodents: what do they model? , 2013, Cell and Tissue Research.

[26]  E. Choleris,et al.  Familiarity and relatedness: Effects on social learning about foods by Norway rats and Mongolian gerbils , 1998 .

[27]  T. Shors Learning during stressful times. , 2004, Learning & memory.

[28]  B. Galef,et al.  Social Influences on the Food Preferences of House Mice (Mus Musculus) , 1989, International Journal of Comparative Psychology.

[29]  F. Hall,et al.  Social deprivation of neonatal, adolescent, and adult rats has distinct neurochemical and behavioral consequences. , 1998, Critical reviews in neurobiology.

[30]  J. Krueger,et al.  Food Restriction Alters the Diurnal Distribution of Sleep in Rats , 1999, Physiology & Behavior.

[31]  Sarah J. Cohen,et al.  Assessing rodent hippocampal involvement in the novel object recognition task. A review , 2015, Behavioural Brain Research.

[32]  B. Galef A case study in behavioral analysis, synthesis and attention to detail: Social learning of food preferences , 2012, Behavioural Brain Research.

[33]  G. Winocur Anterograde and retrograde amnesia in rats with dorsal hippocampal or dorsomedial thalamic lesions , 1990, Behavioural Brain Research.

[34]  C. C. Wrenn,et al.  Social Transmission of Food Preference in Mice , 2004, Current protocols in neuroscience.

[35]  G. Preti,et al.  Carbon disulfide: A semiochemical mediating socially-induced diet choice in rats , 1988, Physiology & Behavior.

[36]  G. Winocur,et al.  Memory consolidation or transformation: context manipulation and hippocampal representations of memory , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[37]  R. Morris,et al.  Hippocampal-neocortical interactions in memory formation, consolidation, and reconsolidation. , 2010, Annual review of psychology.

[38]  H Eichenbaum,et al.  Selective damage to the hippocampal region blocks long‐term retention of a natural and nonspatial stimulus‐stimulus association , 1995, Hippocampus.

[39]  Hall Fs Social Deprivation of Neonatal, Adolescent, and Adult Rats Has Distinct Neurochemical and Behavioral Consequences , 1998 .

[40]  Edith Lesburguères,et al.  Early Tagging of Cortical Networks Is Required for the Formation of Enduring Associative Memory , 2011, Science.

[41]  I. Cuthill,et al.  Improving Bioscience Research Reporting: The ARRIVE Guidelines for Reporting Animal Research † , 2012, Osteoarthritis and cartilage.

[42]  L. Squire,et al.  The neuroscience of remote memory , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[43]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Hippocampal Formation Lesions Impair Performance in an Odor-Odor Association Task Independently of Spatial Context , 2002, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[44]  R. Gerlai Behavioral tests of hippocampal function: simple paradigms complex problems , 2001, Behavioural Brain Research.

[45]  P. Frankland,et al.  The organization of recent and remote memories , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[46]  B. Galef,et al.  Socially transmitted food preferences can be used to study long-term memory in rats , 2003, Learning & behavior.

[47]  J. Crawley,et al.  Social transmission of food preference in mice: methodology and application to galanin-overexpressing transgenic mice. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[48]  Howard Eichenbaum,et al.  The neurobiology of memory based predictions , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.