Specificity of Learning in a Video-Aiming Task: Modifying the Salience of Dynamic Visual Cues
暂无分享,去创建一个
Luc Proteau | Yannick Blandin | Lucette Toussaint | L. Proteau | L. Toussaint | Y. Blandin | Christelle Robin | Christelle Robin | Lucette Toussaint
[1] Luc Proteau,et al. Developmental aspects of the control of manual aiming movements in aligned and non-aligned visual displays , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[2] P. Viviani,et al. Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[3] L. Proteau. Sensory integration in the learning of an aiming task. , 1995, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[4] H Carnahan,et al. What Causes Specificity of Practice in a Manual Aiming Movement: Vision Dominance or Transformation Errors? , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.
[5] P. Fitts. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.
[6] L Tremblay,et al. Specificity of practice: the case of powerlifting. , 1998, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.
[7] Yann Coello,et al. Determination of target distance in a structured environment: Selection of visual information for action , 2000 .
[8] L. Proteau. Chapter 4 On The Specificity of Learning and the Role of Visual Information for Movement Control , 1992 .
[9] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[10] H. Zelaznik,et al. Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. , 1979, Psychological review.
[11] L. Proteau. Visual afferent information dominates other sources of afferent information during mixed practice of a video-aiming task , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[12] D Elliott,et al. Examining the Specificity of Practice Hypothesis: Is Learning Modality Specific? , 2001, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.
[13] L. Proteau,et al. The role of peripheral and central visual information for the directional control of manual aiming movements. , 1999, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[14] Luc Proteau,et al. Planning and control of straight-ahead and angled planar movements in adults and young children. , 2004, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[15] R A Abrams,et al. Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.
[16] Matthew Heath,et al. The control of goal-directed limb movements: Correcting errors in the trajectory , 1999 .
[17] M. Jeannerod,et al. Representation of hand position prior to movement and motor variability. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.
[18] P. Bédard,et al. On-line vs. off-line utilization of peripheral visual afferent information to ensure spatial accuracy of goal-directed movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[19] Zoubin Ghahramani,et al. Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[20] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Short-Term Memory for Reaching to Visual Targets: Psychophysical Evidence for Body-Centered Reference Frames , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[21] L. Proteau,et al. Development of Multiple Movement Representations With Practice: Specificity Versus Flexibility , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.
[22] James L. Lyons,et al. Optimal Control Strategies Under Different Feedback Schedules: Kinematic Evidence , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.
[23] L. Proteau,et al. On the Role of Visual Afferent Information for the Control of Aiming Movements Toward Targets of Different Sizes , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.
[24] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.