Long-range Order in Canary Song

Bird songs range in form from the simple notes of a Chipping Sparrow to the rich performance of the nightingale. Non-adjacent correlations can be found in the syntax of some birdsongs, indicating that the choice of what to sing next is determined not only by the current syllable, but also by previous syllables sung. Here we examine the song of the domesticated canary, a complex singer whose song consists of syllables, grouped into phrases that are arranged in flexible sequences. Phrases are defined by a fundamental time-scale that is independent of the underlying syllable duration. We show that the ordering of phrases is governed by long-range rules: the choice of what phrase to sing next in a given context depends on the history of the song, and for some syllables, highly specific rules produce correlations in song over timescales of up to ten seconds. The neural basis of these long-range correlations may provide insight into how complex behaviors are assembled from more elementary, stereotyped modules.

[1]  F Goller,et al.  Experimental support for a model of birdsong production. , 2003, Physical review. E, Statistical, nonlinear, and soft matter physics.

[2]  Barbara G. Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  Sparse Contour Representations of Sound , 2012, IEEE Signal Processing Letters.

[3]  G. Reeke Marvin Minsky, The Society of Mind , 1991, Artif. Intell..

[4]  H. Güttinger,et al.  Consequences of domestication on the song structures in the canary , 1985 .

[5]  F. Nottebohm,et al.  A comparative study of the behavioral deficits following lesions of various parts of the zebra finch song system: implications for vocal learning , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[6]  Alan Agresti,et al.  Exact conditional tests for cross-classifications: Approximation of attained significance levels , 1979 .

[7]  Dezhe Z. Jin,et al.  Support for a synaptic chain model of neuronal sequence generation , 2010, Nature.

[8]  G. H. Freeman,et al.  Note on an exact treatment of contingency, goodness of fit and other problems of significance. , 1951, Biometrika.

[9]  P. Slater,et al.  SONG ORGANISATION AND SINGING PATTERNS OF THE WILLOW WARBLER, PHYLLOSCOPUS TROCHILUS , 2000 .

[10]  Donald E. Kroodsma,et al.  Vocal Virtuosity in the Brown Thrasher , 1977 .

[11]  M. Magnasco,et al.  Simple motor gestures for birdsongs. , 2001, Physical review letters.

[12]  D. Jin Generating variable birdsong syllable sequences with branching chain networks in avian premotor nucleus HVC. , 2009, Physical review. E, Statistical, nonlinear, and soft matter physics.

[13]  L. Rome,et al.  Superfast Vocal Muscles Control Song Production in Songbirds , 2008, PloS one.

[14]  K. D. Punta,et al.  An ultra-sparse code underlies the generation of neural sequences in a songbird , 2002 .

[15]  Dezhe Z. Jin,et al.  A Compact Statistical Model of the Song Syntax in Bengalese Finch , 2010, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[16]  Masato Okada,et al.  Retrieval of branching sequences in associative memory model with common external input and bias input , 2007 .

[17]  Stefan J. Kiebel,et al.  A Hierarchical Neuronal Model for Generation and Online Recognition of Birdsongs , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[18]  Henrike Hultsch,et al.  How songbirds deal with large amounts of serial information: retrieval rules suggest a hierarchical song memory , 1998, Biological Cybernetics.

[19]  J. Sakata,et al.  Real-Time Contributions of Auditory Feedback to Avian Vocal Motor Control , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  J. F. Prather,et al.  Precise auditory–vocal mirroring in neurons for learned vocal communication , 2008, Nature.

[21]  Clive K. Catchpole,et al.  TEMPORAL AND SEQUENTIAL ORGANISATION OF SONG IN THE SEDGE WARBLER (ACROCEPHALUS SCHOENOBAENUS) , 1976 .

[22]  C. E. Ho,et al.  A procedure for an automated measurement of song similarity , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[23]  Steven Churchill,et al.  The vortex , 1999, SIGGRAPH '99.

[24]  P. J. B. Slater,et al.  Sequences of song in chaffinches , 1983, Animal Behaviour.

[25]  Robert E. Lemon,et al.  Songs of American Redstarts (Setophaga ruticilla): Sequencing Rules and their Relationships to Repertoire Size , 2010 .

[26]  Masato Okada,et al.  Complex Sequencing Rules of Birdsong Can be Explained by Simple Hidden Markov Processes , 2010, PloS one.

[27]  D. Todt,et al.  Song acquisition and acquisition constraints in the nightingale, Luscinia megarhynchos , 1989, Naturwissenschaften.

[28]  H. Güttinger,et al.  The Integration of Learnt and Genetically Programmed Behaviour , 2010 .

[29]  K. Lashley The problem of serial order in behavior , 1951 .

[30]  Markus Diesmann,et al.  A reafferent and feed-forward model of song syntax generation in the Bengalese finch , 2011, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[31]  Golan Yona,et al.  Variations on probabilistic suffix trees: statistical modeling and prediction of protein families , 2001, Bioinform..

[32]  Marcelo O Magnasco,et al.  Sparse time-frequency representations , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  K. Okanoya The Bengalese Finch: A Window on the Behavioral Neurobiology of Birdsong Syntax , 2004, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[34]  S. H. Hulse,et al.  Perceptual mechanisms for individual vocal recognition in European starlings,Sturnus vulgaris , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[35]  Daniel J. Levitin,et al.  Why music moves us , 2010, Nature.

[36]  Kentaro Katahira,et al.  A neural network model for generating complex birdsong syntax , 2007, Biological Cybernetics.

[37]  Felix Naef,et al.  Freedom and Rules: The Acquisition and Reprogramming of a Bird's Learned Song , 2005, Science.

[38]  S. Saporta,et al.  Psycholinguistics: A book of readings , 1964 .

[39]  C. Catchpole,et al.  Song learning in domesticated canaries in a restricted acoustic environment , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[40]  John R. Krebs,et al.  Sequence of songs in repertoires of western meadowlarks (Sturnella neglecta) , 1975 .

[41]  Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al.  Probabilistic Analysis of an Ancient Undeciphered Script , 2010, Computer.

[42]  Dana Ron,et al.  The power of amnesia: Learning probabilistic automata with variable memory length , 1996, Machine Learning.

[43]  J. Fodor Précis of The Modularity of Mind , 1985, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[44]  Dai Watanabe,et al.  Neural Coding of Syntactic Structure in Learned Vocalizations in the Songbird , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Tetsuro Nishino,et al.  Ethological data mining: an automata-based approach to extract behavioral units and rules , 2009, Data Mining and Knowledge Discovery.