Long-range Order in Canary Song
暂无分享,去创建一个
Timothy J. Gardner | Jeffrey E. Markowitz | Elizabeth Ivie | Laura Kligler | J. Markowitz | T. Gardner | Elizabeth Ivie | Laura Kligler
[1] F Goller,et al. Experimental support for a model of birdsong production. , 2003, Physical review. E, Statistical, nonlinear, and soft matter physics.
[2] Barbara G. Shinn-Cunningham,et al. Sparse Contour Representations of Sound , 2012, IEEE Signal Processing Letters.
[3] G. Reeke. Marvin Minsky, The Society of Mind , 1991, Artif. Intell..
[4] H. Güttinger,et al. Consequences of domestication on the song structures in the canary , 1985 .
[5] F. Nottebohm,et al. A comparative study of the behavioral deficits following lesions of various parts of the zebra finch song system: implications for vocal learning , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[6] Alan Agresti,et al. Exact conditional tests for cross-classifications: Approximation of attained significance levels , 1979 .
[7] Dezhe Z. Jin,et al. Support for a synaptic chain model of neuronal sequence generation , 2010, Nature.
[8] G. H. Freeman,et al. Note on an exact treatment of contingency, goodness of fit and other problems of significance. , 1951, Biometrika.
[9] P. Slater,et al. SONG ORGANISATION AND SINGING PATTERNS OF THE WILLOW WARBLER, PHYLLOSCOPUS TROCHILUS , 2000 .
[10] Donald E. Kroodsma,et al. Vocal Virtuosity in the Brown Thrasher , 1977 .
[11] M. Magnasco,et al. Simple motor gestures for birdsongs. , 2001, Physical review letters.
[12] D. Jin. Generating variable birdsong syllable sequences with branching chain networks in avian premotor nucleus HVC. , 2009, Physical review. E, Statistical, nonlinear, and soft matter physics.
[13] L. Rome,et al. Superfast Vocal Muscles Control Song Production in Songbirds , 2008, PloS one.
[14] K. D. Punta,et al. An ultra-sparse code underlies the generation of neural sequences in a songbird , 2002 .
[15] Dezhe Z. Jin,et al. A Compact Statistical Model of the Song Syntax in Bengalese Finch , 2010, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[16] Masato Okada,et al. Retrieval of branching sequences in associative memory model with common external input and bias input , 2007 .
[17] Stefan J. Kiebel,et al. A Hierarchical Neuronal Model for Generation and Online Recognition of Birdsongs , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[18] Henrike Hultsch,et al. How songbirds deal with large amounts of serial information: retrieval rules suggest a hierarchical song memory , 1998, Biological Cybernetics.
[19] J. Sakata,et al. Real-Time Contributions of Auditory Feedback to Avian Vocal Motor Control , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[20] J. F. Prather,et al. Precise auditory–vocal mirroring in neurons for learned vocal communication , 2008, Nature.
[21] Clive K. Catchpole,et al. TEMPORAL AND SEQUENTIAL ORGANISATION OF SONG IN THE SEDGE WARBLER (ACROCEPHALUS SCHOENOBAENUS) , 1976 .
[22] C. E. Ho,et al. A procedure for an automated measurement of song similarity , 2000, Animal Behaviour.
[23] Steven Churchill,et al. The vortex , 1999, SIGGRAPH '99.
[24] P. J. B. Slater,et al. Sequences of song in chaffinches , 1983, Animal Behaviour.
[25] Robert E. Lemon,et al. Songs of American Redstarts (Setophaga ruticilla): Sequencing Rules and their Relationships to Repertoire Size , 2010 .
[26] Masato Okada,et al. Complex Sequencing Rules of Birdsong Can be Explained by Simple Hidden Markov Processes , 2010, PloS one.
[27] D. Todt,et al. Song acquisition and acquisition constraints in the nightingale, Luscinia megarhynchos , 1989, Naturwissenschaften.
[28] H. Güttinger,et al. The Integration of Learnt and Genetically Programmed Behaviour , 2010 .
[29] K. Lashley. The problem of serial order in behavior , 1951 .
[30] Markus Diesmann,et al. A reafferent and feed-forward model of song syntax generation in the Bengalese finch , 2011, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.
[31] Golan Yona,et al. Variations on probabilistic suffix trees: statistical modeling and prediction of protein families , 2001, Bioinform..
[32] Marcelo O Magnasco,et al. Sparse time-frequency representations , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[33] K. Okanoya. The Bengalese Finch: A Window on the Behavioral Neurobiology of Birdsong Syntax , 2004, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[34] S. H. Hulse,et al. Perceptual mechanisms for individual vocal recognition in European starlings,Sturnus vulgaris , 1998, Animal Behaviour.
[35] Daniel J. Levitin,et al. Why music moves us , 2010, Nature.
[36] Kentaro Katahira,et al. A neural network model for generating complex birdsong syntax , 2007, Biological Cybernetics.
[37] Felix Naef,et al. Freedom and Rules: The Acquisition and Reprogramming of a Bird's Learned Song , 2005, Science.
[38] S. Saporta,et al. Psycholinguistics: A book of readings , 1964 .
[39] C. Catchpole,et al. Song learning in domesticated canaries in a restricted acoustic environment , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[40] John R. Krebs,et al. Sequence of songs in repertoires of western meadowlarks (Sturnella neglecta) , 1975 .
[41] Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al. Probabilistic Analysis of an Ancient Undeciphered Script , 2010, Computer.
[42] Dana Ron,et al. The power of amnesia: Learning probabilistic automata with variable memory length , 1996, Machine Learning.
[43] J. Fodor. Précis of The Modularity of Mind , 1985, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[44] Dai Watanabe,et al. Neural Coding of Syntactic Structure in Learned Vocalizations in the Songbird , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[45] Tetsuro Nishino,et al. Ethological data mining: an automata-based approach to extract behavioral units and rules , 2009, Data Mining and Knowledge Discovery.