Capacity limits during early perceptual encoding

When a unique stimulus is embedded in an otherwise homogenous display, it is thought to “pop-out” due to its relative increase in salience. We investigated whether the visual system has the ability to equally prioritize multiple salient pop-out items competing for awareness or whether this early stage of visual processing is constrained by capacity limits. We used signal detection (dV) methods to determine if sensitivity to a salient pop-out item decreases as function of the number of total salient items present in the visual display. Participants engaged in a signal detection task where they had to report the presence or absence of a simple display change that involved either a pop-out or a non-salient distractor stimulus. Results across four experiments consistently showed that sensitivity to changes involving pop-out stimuli was significantly reduced after more than one of these items was present in the visual array. Results are discussed in terms of neural models of visual encoding and other known capacity limits during visual processing.

[1]  Ana Torralbo,et al.  Perceptual-Load-Induced Selection as a Result of Local Competitive Interactions in Visual Cortex , 2008, Psychological science.

[2]  Henry Railo,et al.  The role of attention in subitizing , 2008, Cognition.

[3]  Caren M Rotello,et al.  Type I error rates and power analyses for single-point sensitivity measures , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  Richard L. Lewis,et al.  The mind and brain of short-term memory. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[5]  G. Alvarez,et al.  How many locations can be selected at once? , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  B. C. Motter,et al.  The roles of cortical image separation and size in active visual search performance. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[7]  Rufin VanRullen,et al.  Spacing affects some but not all visual searches: implications for theories of attention and crowding. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[8]  S. Kastner,et al.  Stimulus context modulates competition in human extrastriate cortex , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  R. Marois,et al.  Capacity limits of information processing in the brain , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  The Capacity of Visual Short-Term Memory is Set Both by Visual Information Load and by Number of Objects , 2004, Psychological science.

[11]  R. Abrams,et al.  Motion Onset Captures Attention , 2003, Psychological science.

[12]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Single-trial classification of parallel pre-attentive and serial attentive processes using functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[13]  N. Cowan The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[14]  G. Woodman,et al.  Storage of features, conjunctions and objects in visual working memory. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Attentional and oculomotor capture by onset, luminance and color singletons , 2000, Vision Research.

[16]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Response profiles to texture border patterns in area V1 , 2000, Visual Neuroscience.

[17]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  Attention improves or impairs visual performance by enhancing spatial resolution , 1998, Nature.

[18]  Hugh Garavan,et al.  Serial attention within working memory , 1998, Memory & cognition.

[19]  Edward K. Vogel,et al.  The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions , 1997, Nature.

[20]  G R Mangun,et al.  Spatial distribution of visual attention: Perceptual sensitivity and response latency , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  J. Theeuwes Abrupt luminance change pops out; abrupt color change does not , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  S. Yantis Stimulus-Driven Attentional Capture , 1993 .

[23]  E. Miller,et al.  Suppression of visual responses of neurons in inferior temporal cortex of the awake macaque by addition of a second stimulus , 1993, Brain Research.

[24]  S. Yantis Stimulus-driven attentional capture and attentional control settings. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[27]  H Pashler,et al.  Familiarity and visual change detection , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  S. Yantis,et al.  Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  A. Treisman Preattentive processing in vision , 1985, Comput. Vis. Graph. Image Process..

[30]  B. Julesz,et al.  Human factors and behavioral science: Textons, the fundamental elements in preattentive vision and perception of textures , 1983, The Bell System Technical Journal.

[31]  J E Hoffman,et al.  A two-stage model of visual search , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  H. Egeth Parallel versus serial processes in multidimensional stimulus discrimination , 1966 .

[33]  E. M. Jensen,et al.  The subitizing and counting of visually presented fields of dots. , 1950 .

[34]  Steven Yantis,et al.  Attentional capture by abrupt onsets: new perceptual objects or visual masking? , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[36]  Z. Pylyshyn,et al.  Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. , 1994, Psychological review.

[37]  H. Egeth,et al.  Does the identification of simple features require serial processing? , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[38]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[39]  M. Keeling,et al.  Involuntary orienting to sound improves visual perception , 2022 .