Electrical Neuroimaging Reveals Timing of Attentional Control Activity in Human Brain

Voluntarily shifting attention to a location of the visual field improves the perception of events that occur there. Regions of frontal cortex are thought to provide the top-down control signal that initiates a shift of attention, but because of the temporal limitations of functional brain imaging, the timing and sequence of attentional-control operations remain unknown. We used a new analytical technique (beamformer spatial filtering) to reconstruct the anatomical sources of low-frequency brain waves in humans associated with attentional control across time. Following a signal to shift attention, control activity was seen in parietal cortex 100–200 ms before activity was seen in frontal cortex. Parietal cortex was then reactivated prior to anticipatory biasing of activity in occipital cortex. The magnitudes of early parietal activations were strongly predictive of the degree of attentional improvement in perceptual performance. These results show that parietal cortex, not frontal cortex, provides the initial signals to shift attention and indicate that top-down attentional control is not purely top down.

[1]  S. Yantis,et al.  Control of Attention Shifts between Vision and Audition in Human Cortex , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Steven L. Miller,et al.  Neural Processes Involved in Directing Attention , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  S. Nieuwenhuis,et al.  The orienting of visuospatial attention: an event-related brain potential study. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[4]  A. Schnitzler,et al.  Dynamic imaging of coherent sources: Studying neural interactions in the human brain. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  L. M. Ward,et al.  Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  A. Dale,et al.  Functional Parcellation of Attentional Control Regions of the Brain , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  G. R Mangun,et al.  Shifting visual attention in space: an electrophysiological analysis using high spatial resolution mapping , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[8]  S. Yantis,et al.  Transient neural activity in human parietal cortex during spatial attention shifts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  Sarah Shomstein,et al.  Parietal Cortex Mediates Voluntary Control of Spatial and Nonspatial Auditory Attention , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  D. Gitelman,et al.  Functional Specificity of Superior Parietal Mediation of Spatial Shifting , 2001, NeuroImage.

[11]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  α-Band Electroencephalographic Activity over Occipital Cortex Indexes Visuospatial Attention Bias and Predicts Visual Target Detection , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  M. Corbetta,et al.  An Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Orienting of Attention , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  Jessica J. Green,et al.  Lateralized frontal activity elicited by attention-directing visual and auditory cues. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[14]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[15]  R. Desimone,et al.  Gamma-band synchronization in visual cortex predicts speed of change detection , 2006, Nature.

[16]  R. Verleger,et al.  Spatiotemporal overlap between brain activation related to saccade preparation and attentional orienting , 2006, Brain Research.

[17]  S. Yantis,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of feature-based attentional control. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[18]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modelling , 2003, NeuroImage.

[19]  S. Yantis,et al.  Spatially selective representations of voluntary and stimulus-driven attentional priority in human occipital, parietal, and frontal cortex. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  P. Bandettini,et al.  Understanding neural system dynamics through task modulation and measurement of functional MRI amplitude, latency, and width , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  W. Drongelen,et al.  Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial filtering , 1997, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[22]  Joel R. Meyer,et al.  A large-scale distributed network for covert spatial attention: further anatomical delineation based on stringent behavioural and cognitive controls. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[24]  Manuel Schabus,et al.  Fronto-parietal EEG coherence in theta and upper alpha reflect central executive functions of working memory. , 2005, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[25]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Erratum to “Translocation machinery for synthesis of integral membrane and secretory proteins in dendritic spines” , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[27]  Marty G Woldorff,et al.  Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention , 2007, PLoS biology.

[28]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[29]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  Jessica J. Green,et al.  An event-related potential study of supramodal attentional control and crossmodal attention effects. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[31]  G. V. Simpson,et al.  Anticipatory Biasing of Visuospatial Attention Indexed by Retinotopically Specific α-Bank Electroencephalography Increases over Occipital Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  A. von Stein,et al.  Different frequencies for different scales of cortical integration: from local gamma to long range alpha/theta synchronization. , 2000, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[33]  B Giesbrecht,et al.  Neural mechanisms of top-down control during spatial and feature attention , 2003, NeuroImage.

[34]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[35]  A. Nobre,et al.  The dynamics of shifting visuospatial attention revealed by event-related potentials , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  E. Knudsen Fundamental components of attention. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[37]  Peter Praamstra,et al.  Frontoparietal control of spatial attention and motor intention in human EEG. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  R W Cox,et al.  Software tools for analysis and visualization of fMRI data , 1997, NMR in biomedicine.

[39]  M. Berger,et al.  High Gamma Power Is Phase-Locked to Theta Oscillations in Human Neocortex , 2006, Science.

[40]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The physiological basis of attentional modulation in extrastriate visual areas , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[41]  F. Gonzalez-Lima,et al.  Structural equation modeling and its application to network analysis in functional brain imaging , 1994 .

[42]  G. Mangun,et al.  The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[43]  Geoffrey F. Woodman,et al.  Electrophysiological measurement of rapid shifts of attention during visual search , 1999, Nature.

[44]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[45]  Gareth R. Barnes,et al.  Group imaging of task-related changes in cortical synchronisation using nonparametric permutation testing , 2003, NeuroImage.

[46]  J. Jonides,et al.  Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[47]  G. R. Mangun,et al.  Monitoring the Visual World: Hemispheric Asymmetries and Subcortical Processes in Attention , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  Karsten Hoechstetter,et al.  BESA Source Coherence: A New Method to Study Cortical Oscillatory Coupling , 2003, Brain Topography.