Competitive Interactions of Attentional Resources in Early Visual Cortex during Sustained Visuospatial Attention within or between Visual Hemifields: Evidence for the Different-hemifield Advantage
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Mark M Schira,et al. Interactions between task difficulty and hemispheric distribution of attended locations: implications for the splitting attention debate. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[2] Jenna G. Kelly,et al. Bilateral attentional advantage on elementary visual tasks , 2009, Vision Research.
[3] John J. Foxe,et al. Visuo-spatial neural response interactions in early cortical processing during a simple reaction time task: a high-density electrical mapping study , 2001, Neuropsychologia.
[4] Cheuk Yee Pang,et al. Competitive interactions in somatosensory cortex for concurrent vibrotactile stimulation between and within hands , 2015, Biological Psychology.
[5] Satoshi Shioiri,et al. Asymmetrical brain activity induced by voluntary spatial attention depends on the visual hemifield: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study , 2011, Brain and Cognition.
[6] P. Cavanagh,et al. Flexible cognitive resources: competitive content maps for attention and memory , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[7] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[8] S. Hillyard,et al. Selective attention to stimulus location modulates the steady-state visual evoked potential. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[9] Tutis Vilis,et al. fMRI reveals greater within‐ than between‐hemifield integration in the human lateral occipital cortex , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.
[10] S. A. Hillyard,et al. Sustained division of the attentional spotlight , 2003, Nature.
[11] Carrie J. McAdams,et al. Effects of Attention on Orientation-Tuning Functions of Single Neurons in Macaque Cortical Area V4 , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[12] D. Regan. Human brain electrophysiology: Evoked potentials and evoked magnetic fields in science and medicine , 1989 .
[13] Stefan Treue,et al. Expansion of MT Neurons Excitatory Receptive Fields during Covert Attentive Tracking , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[14] P. Cavanagh,et al. Anatomical constraints on attention: hemifield independence is a signature of multifocal spatial selection. , 2012, Journal of vision.
[15] S. Pollmann,et al. Interhemispheric resource sharing: Decreasing benefits with increasing processing efficiency , 2005, Brain and Cognition.
[16] J. Liederman,et al. Developmental changes in hemispheric independence. , 1985, Child development.
[17] Sabine Kastner,et al. Visual attention as a multilevel selection process , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[18] L. Kehrer. Central performance drop on perceptual segregation tasks. , 1989, Spatial vision.
[19] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. Bilateral field advantage in visual crowding , 2009, Vision Research.
[20] R. Desimone,et al. Competitive Mechanisms Subserve Attention in Macaque Areas V2 and V4 , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[21] S. Brandt,et al. Hemifield effects of spatial attention in early human visual cortex , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.
[22] Matthias M. Müller,et al. Effects of spatial selective attention on the steady-state visual evoked potential in the 20-28 Hz range. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[23] S. Kastner,et al. Stimulus similarity modulates competitive interactions in human visual cortex. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[24] Claus Bundesen,et al. Visual attention capacity parameters covary with hemifield alignment , 2013, Neuropsychologia.
[25] Steven A. Hillyard,et al. Tracking the allocation of attention in visual scenes with steady-state evoked potentials , 2011 .
[26] Rufin VanRullen,et al. Bullet trains and steam engines: exogenous attention zips but endogenous attention chugs along. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[27] G. Humphreys,et al. Bilateral Field Advantage in Visual Enumeration , 2011, PLoS ONE.
[28] John K. Tsotsos,et al. The selective tuning model of attention: psychophysical evidence for a suppressive annulus around an attended item , 2003, Vision Research.
[29] W. Brown,et al. Bilateral field interactions, hemispheric specialization and evoked potential interhemispheric transmission time , 1997, Neuropsychologia.
[30] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. The neural basis of biased competition in human visual cortex , 2001, Neuropsychologia.
[31] Mieke Donk,et al. Detection Performance in Pop-Out Tasks: Nonmonotonic Changes with Display Size and Eccentricity , 2002, Perception.
[32] S. Pollmann,et al. The neural basis of the bilateral distribution advantage , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.
[33] B. Rockstroh,et al. Statistical control of artifacts in dense array EEG/MEG studies. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[34] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[35] A Belger,et al. Costs and benefits of integrating information between the cerebral hemispheres: a computational perspective. , 1998, Neuropsychology.
[36] Matthias M. Müller,et al. Keeping focused: Sustained spatial selective visual attention is maintained in healthy old age , 2012, Brain Research.
[37] D. Kahneman,et al. Attention and Effort , 1973 .
[38] S Plainis,et al. Raised Visual Detection Thresholds Depend on the Level of Complexity of Cognitive Foveal Loading , 2001, Perception.
[39] R. Marois,et al. Capacity limits of information processing in the brain , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[40] Steven A. Hillyard,et al. Independent hemispheric attentional systems mediate visual search in split-brain patients , 1989, Nature.
[41] Matthias M. Müller,et al. The time course of cortical facilitation during cued shifts of spatial attention , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[42] S. Brandt,et al. What determines sustained visual attention? The impact of distracter positions, task difficulty and visual fields compared , 2007, Brain Research.
[43] Matthias M. Müller,et al. Sustained division of spatial attention to multiple locations within one hemifield , 2007, Neuroscience Letters.
[44] Malcolm A. Jeeves,et al. Bilateral visual field processing and evoked potential interhemispheric transmission time , 1993, Neuropsychologia.
[45] M. Corbetta,et al. Spatial neglect and attention networks. , 2011, Annual review of neuroscience.
[46] M. Carrasco,et al. The locus of attentional effects in texture segmentation , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[47] Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al. Blind separation and blind deconvolution: an information-theoretic approach , 1995, 1995 International Conference on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing.
[48] Eileen Kowler,et al. Attentional interference at small spatial separations , 1999, Vision Research.
[49] S. Shipp. Interhemispheric integration in visual search , 2011, Neuropsychologia.
[50] H. Pashler,et al. Evidence for split attentional foci. , 2000, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.
[51] Matthias M. Müller,et al. Sustained Splits of Attention within versus across Visual Hemifields Produce Distinct Spatial Gain Profiles , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[52] Edward K. Vogel,et al. The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions , 1997, Nature.
[53] A. Belger,et al. Interhemispheric interaction affected by computational complexity , 1992, Neuropsychologia.
[54] Suzanne E. Welcome,et al. How dynamic is interhemispheric interaction? Effects of task switching on the across-hemisphere advantage , 2008, Brain and Cognition.
[55] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[56] G. Sperling,et al. Dynamics of automatic and controlled visual attention. , 1987, Science.
[57] Stephen M. Kosslyn,et al. Discrimination within and between hemifields: A new constraint on theories of attention , 1991, Neuropsychologia.
[58] M. Banich. The Missing Link: The Role of Interhemispheric Interaction in Attentional Processing , 1998, Brain and Cognition.
[59] Marisa Carrasco,et al. Attention improves or impairs visual performance by enhancing spatial resolution , 1998, Nature.
[60] A. Friedman,et al. Multiple resources in divided attention: a cross-modal test of the independence of hemispheric resources. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[61] D. Spinelli,et al. Spatiotemporal analysis of the cortical sources of the steady‐state visual evoked potential , 2007, Human brain mapping.
[62] J. Duhamel,et al. The Spatial and Temporal Deployment of Voluntary Attention across the Visual Field , 2009, PloS one.
[63] John K. Tsotsos,et al. Modeling Visual Attention via Selective Tuning , 1995, Artif. Intell..
[64] John K. Tsotsos,et al. The center-surround profile of the focus of attention arises from recurrent processing in visual cortex. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.
[65] S. Andersen,et al. Effects of overt and covert attention on the steady-state visual evoked potential , 2012, Neuroscience Letters.
[66] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.
[67] Søren K. Andersen,et al. Attentional bias of competitive interactions in neuronal networks of early visual processing in the human brain , 2008, NeuroImage.
[68] P. Cavanagh,et al. Independent Resources for Attentional Tracking in the Left and Right Visual Hemifields , 2005, Psychological science.
[69] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Mechanisms of directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by functional MRI. , 1998, Science.
[70] R. Desimone,et al. Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. , 1985, Science.
[71] Kimron Shapiro,et al. Direct measurement of attentional dwell time in human vision , 1994, Nature.
[72] T. Ludwig,et al. Hemispheric interactions: The Bilateral Advantage and Task Difficulty , 1992, Cortex.
[73] S. Palmer,et al. A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception: I. Perceptual grouping and figure-ground organization. , 2012, Psychological bulletin.
[74] L. Kehrer. Perceptual segregation and retinal position. , 1987, Spatial vision.
[75] L. Kehrer,et al. A central performance drop with luminance stimuli requiring spatial integration , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.
[76] John K. Tsotsos,et al. Attending to orientation results in an inhibitory surround in orientation space , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.
[77] R. Dolan,et al. Attentional load and sensory competition in human vision: modulation of fMRI responses by load at fixation during task-irrelevant stimulation in the peripheral visual field. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.