Recommender systems, consumer preferences, and anchoring effects

Recommender systems are becoming a salient part of many e-commerce websites. Much research has focused on advancing recommendation technologies to improve the accuracy of predictions, while behavioral aspects of using recommender systems are often overlooked. In this study, we explore how consumer preferences at the time of consumption are impacted by predictions generated by recommender systems. We conducted three controlled laboratory experiments to explore the effects of system recommendations on preferences. Studies 1 and 2 investigated user preferences for television programs, which were surveyed immediately following program viewing. Study 3 broadened to an additional context—preferences for jokes. Results provide strong evidence viewers’ preferences are malleable and can be significantly influenced by ratings provided by recommender systems. Additionally, the effects of pure number-based anchoring can be separated from the effects of the perceived reliability of a recommender system. Finally, the effect of anchoring is roughly continuous, operating over a range of perturbations of the system.

[1]  Brian H. Bornstein,et al.  The More You Ask For, the More You Get: Anchoring in Personal Injury Verdicts , 1996 .

[2]  Sean M. McNee,et al.  Interfaces for Eliciting New User Preferences in Recommender Systems , 2003, User Modeling.

[3]  George Karypis,et al.  Item-based top-N recommendation algorithms , 2004, TOIS.

[4]  Kirsten Swearingen,et al.  Beyond Algorithms: An HCI Perspective on Recommender Systems , 2001 .

[5]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Measures of Anchoring in Estimation Tasks , 1995 .

[6]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[7]  G. Northcraft,et al.  Experts, amateurs, and real estate: An anchoring-and-adjustment perspective on property pricing decisions , 1987 .

[8]  Gediminas Adomavicius,et al.  Toward the next generation of recommender systems: a survey of the state-of-the-art and possible extensions , 2005, IEEE Transactions on Knowledge and Data Engineering.

[9]  J. K. Murnighan,et al.  Starting low but ending high: a reversal of the anchoring effect in auctions. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  Johnnie E.V. Johnson,et al.  To What Extent do Investors in a Financial Market Anchor Their Judgments? Evidence from the Hong Kong Horserace Betting Market , 2009 .

[11]  David Heckerman,et al.  Empirical Analysis of Predictive Algorithms for Collaborative Filtering , 1998, UAI.

[12]  Todd J. Thorsteinson,et al.  Anchoring effects on performance judgments , 2008 .

[13]  Thomas Mussweiler,et al.  Numeric Judgments under Uncertainty: The Role of Knowledge in Anchoring , 2000 .

[14]  John Riedl,et al.  Is seeing believing?: how recommender system interfaces affect users' opinions , 2003, CHI '03.

[15]  Yehuda Koren,et al.  Improved Neighborhood-based Collaborative Filtering , 2007 .

[16]  Eric J. Johnson,et al.  Incorporating the Irrelevant: Anchors in Judgments of Belief and Value , 2002 .

[17]  P. Slovic The Construction of Preference , 1995 .

[18]  John Riedl,et al.  Item-based collaborative filtering recommendation algorithms , 2001, WWW '01.

[19]  Yehuda Koren,et al.  Matrix Factorization Techniques for Recommender Systems , 2009, Computer.

[20]  Izak Benbasat,et al.  The Effects of Personalizaion and Familiarity on Trust and Adoption of Recommendation Agents , 2006, MIS Q..

[21]  James Bennett,et al.  The Netflix Prize , 2007 .

[22]  J. Edward Russo,et al.  Understanding the effect of a numerical anchor , 2010 .

[23]  F. Strack,et al.  Explaining the Enigmatic Anchoring Effect: Mechanisms of Selective Accessibility , 1997 .

[24]  Li Chen,et al.  Trust-inspiring explanation interfaces for recommender systems , 2007, Knowl. Based Syst..

[25]  Kenneth Y. Goldberg,et al.  Eigentaste: A Constant Time Collaborative Filtering Algorithm , 2001, Information Retrieval.