Effect of same- and different-modality spatial cues on auditory and visual target identification.

A target identification paradigm was used to study cross-modal spatial cuing effects on auditory and visual target identification. Each trial consisted of an auditory or visual spatial cue followed by an auditory or visual target. The cue and target could be either of the same modality (within-modality conditions) or of different modalities (between-modalities conditions). In 3 experiments, a larger cue validity effect was apparent on within-modality trials than on between-modalities trials. In addition, the likelihood of identifying a significant cross-modal cuing effect was observed to depend on the predictability of the cue-target relation. These effects are interpreted as evidence (a) of separate auditory and visual spatial attention mechanisms and (b) that target identification may be influenced by spatial cues of another modality but that this effect is primarily dependent on the engagement of endogenous attentional mechanisms.

[1]  L. M. Ward,et al.  Cross-modal control of attention shifts , 1998 .

[2]  M. Posner Chronometric explorations of mind : the third Paul M. Fitts lectures, delivered at the University of Michigan, September 1976 , 1978 .

[3]  G. C. Thompson,et al.  Brain stem auditory pathways involved in reflexive head orientation to sound. , 1978, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  M. Farah,et al.  Parietal lobe mechanisms of spatial attention: Modality-specific or supramodal? , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Extrageniculate Contributions to Reflex Visual Orienting in Normal Humans: A Temporal Hemifield Advantage , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  J. Henderson,et al.  The spatial distribution of attention following an exogenous cue , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  David L. Sparks,et al.  Auditory receptive fields in primate superior colliculus shift with changes in eye position , 1984, Nature.

[8]  J. Duncan,et al.  Beyond the search surface: visual search and attentional engagement. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  B. Pillon,et al.  Time for reorienting of attention: A premotor hypothesis of the underlying mechanism , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  L. M. Ward,et al.  Supramodal and modality-specific mechanisms for stimulus-driven shifts of auditory and visual attention. , 1994, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[11]  H. Egeth,et al.  Further evidence for a time-independent shift of the focus of attention , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  H. Buchtel,et al.  Spatial attentional shifts: Implications for the role of polysensory mechanisms , 1988, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Howard S. Bashinski,et al.  Enhancement of perceptual sensitivity as the result of selectively attending to spatial locations , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[14]  J. Findlay,et al.  The effect of visual attention on peripheral discrimination thresholds in single and multiple element displays. , 1988, Acta psychologica.

[15]  K. Alho,et al.  Intermodal selective attention. I. Effects on event-related potentials to lateralized auditory and visual stimuli. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[16]  T A Mondor,et al.  On the relation between auditory spatial attention and auditory perceptual asymmetries , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[17]  A. Treisman Contextual Cues in Selective Listening , 1960 .

[18]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Shifting and focusing auditory spatial attention. , 1995 .

[19]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Constraints on the selection of auditory information. , 1998 .

[20]  F. Michèl,et al.  Verbal visual extinction in right/left hemisphere lesion patients and the problem of lexical access , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  Jon Driver,et al.  Covert Spatial Orienting in Audition: Exogenous and Endogenous Mechanisms , 1994 .

[22]  J. Duncan,et al.  Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.

[23]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A PET study of visuospatial attention , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  J. Driver,et al.  Audiovisual links in exogenous covert spatial orienting , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  P. Rabbitt,et al.  Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption , 1989 .

[26]  K. Heilman,et al.  Auditory neglect in man. , 1972, Archives of neurology.

[27]  D. Denny-Brown,et al.  The significance of perceptual rivalry resulting from parietal lesion. , 1952, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[28]  M. Posner,et al.  Visual dominance: an information-processing account of its origins and significance. , 1976, Psychological review.

[29]  D. Broadbent Perception and communication , 1958 .

[30]  T A Mondor,et al.  Inhibitory processes in auditory selective attention: Evidence of location-based and frequency-based inhibition of return , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.

[31]  E. Renzi,et al.  Auditory extinction following hemisphere damage , 1984, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  M. P. Bryden,et al.  Orienting of auditory spatial attention: Effects of a lateralized tone cue , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  Nigel Harvey,et al.  Non-Informative Effects of Stimuli Functioning as Cues , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[34]  J. R. Simon Ear preference in a simple reaction-time task. , 1967, Journal of experimental psychology.

[35]  Y. Tsal,et al.  Location dominance in attending to color and shape. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  E. John,et al.  Evoked-Potential Correlates of Stimulus Uncertainty , 1965, Science.

[37]  M. Posner Foundations of cognitive science , 1989 .