On the Interpretation of Onsets of Auditory Context Effects in Word Production

Abstract Six experiments are reported in which participants named pictures that were accompanied by auditorily presented distractors. Distractors that consisted of the first phonemes of the target names caused faster target-naming times than unrelated controls in a wide range of stimulus-onset asynchronies (SOAs), including SOA −300 ms. In addition, two variables that influenced the onset of the phonological effect obtained with word distractors were identified, one of which was the presence or absence of a semantic manipulation in the experiment. The results (a) show that data patterns that had seemed to support strict-serial models of language production (Schriefers, Meyer, & Levelt, 1990) are in fact not univocally interpretable and (b) highlight that the interpretation of (onsets of) context effects cannot be made without clear assumptions about both language production and language perception.

[1]  P A Starreveld,et al.  Word substitution errors in a speeded picture-word task. , 1999, The American journal of psychology.

[2]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Lexical Representation and Process , 1991 .

[3]  Pienie Zwitserlood Access to phonological-form representations in language comprehension and production. , 1994 .

[4]  M. C. Smith,et al.  Tracing the time course of picture--word processing. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[5]  K Rayner,et al.  Graphemic and semantic similarity effects in the picture-word interference task. , 1986, British journal of psychology.

[6]  G. Dell,et al.  Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. , 1997, Psychological review.

[7]  Kathryn Bock,et al.  Language production : Grammatical encoding , 1994 .

[8]  M. F. Garrett,et al.  The Analysis of Sentence Production1 , 1975 .

[9]  Wido La Heij,et al.  THE LOCUS OF ORTHOGRAPHIC-PHONOLOGICAL FACILITATION : REPLY TO ROELOFS, MEYER, AND LEVELT (1996) , 1996 .

[10]  M. Fayol,et al.  Writing words from pictures: What representations are activated, and when? , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[11]  G S Dell,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.

[12]  A. Roelofs,et al.  The WEAVER model of word-form encoding in speech production , 1997, Cognition.

[13]  Markus F. Damian,et al.  Semantic and phonological codes interact in single word production. , 1999 .

[14]  H. Schriefers,et al.  Phonological facilitation in picture-word interference experiments: Effects of stimulus onset asynchrony and types of interfering stimuli. , 1991 .

[15]  W. Levelt,et al.  Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .

[16]  D. G. MacKay,et al.  Output editing for lexical status in artificially elicited slips of the tongue , 1975 .

[17]  A. Roelofs,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking , 1992, Cognition.

[18]  Zenzi M. Griffin,et al.  Constraint, Word Frequency, and the Relationship between Lexical Processing Levels in Spoken Word Production , 1998 .

[19]  M. Potter,et al.  Time to understand pictures and words , 1975, Nature.

[20]  G. Dell,et al.  Stages of lexical access in language production , 1992, Cognition.

[21]  B. Keysar,et al.  When do speakers take into account common ground? , 1996, Cognition.

[22]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The TRACE model of speech perception , 1986, Cognitive Psychology.

[23]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The relation between syntactic and phonological knowledge in lexical access: evidence from the `tip-of-the-tongue' phenomenon , 1997, Cognition.

[24]  P. Starreveld,et al.  Time-course analysis of semantic and orthographic context effects in picture naming. , 1996 .

[25]  K. Rayner,et al.  Visual.vs. phonemic contributions to the importance of the initial letter in word identification , 1978 .

[26]  Victoria A. Fromkin,et al.  The Non-Anomalous Nature of Anomalous Utterances , 1971 .

[27]  Wido La Heij,et al.  Components of Stroop-like interference in picture naming , 1988 .

[28]  Trevor A. Harley,et al.  A Critique of Top-down Independent Levels Models of Speech Production: Evidence from Non-plan-Internal Speech Errors , 1984, Cogn. Sci..

[29]  P. Starreveld,et al.  What about phonological facilitation, response-set membership, and phonological coactivation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[30]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  The time course of lexical access in speech production: A study of picture naming , 1991 .

[31]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  Multiple perspectives on word production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[32]  Trevor A. Harley,et al.  Phonological activation of semantic competitors during lexical access in speech production , 1993 .

[33]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Activation, competition, and frequency in lexical access , 1991 .

[34]  R. Golinkoff,et al.  Automatic semantic processing in a picture-word interference task. , 1975 .

[35]  Nadine Martin,et al.  Phonological Facilitation of Semantic Errors in Normal and Aphasic Speakers , 1996 .

[36]  Colin M. Brown,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence on the time course of semantic and phonological processes in speech production. , 1997 .

[37]  K. Rayner,et al.  Visual-feature and response components in a picture-word interference task with beginning and skilled readers. , 1977, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[38]  G. Dell,et al.  Mediated and convergent lexical priming in language production: a comment on Levelt et al. (1991). , 1991, Psychological review.

[39]  R. R. Peterson,et al.  Lexical selection and phonological encoding during language production: Evidence for cascaded processing. , 1998 .

[40]  P. Briggs,et al.  Phonological coding in good and poor readers. , 1982, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[41]  N. Martin,et al.  Variables influencing the occurrence of naming errors: Implications for models of lexical retrieval☆ , 1989 .

[42]  Gerard Kempen,et al.  The lexicalization process in sentence production and naming: indirect election of words , 1983, Cognition.

[43]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[44]  W. Glaser,et al.  The time course of picture-word interference. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[45]  W. Levelt,et al.  Monitoring and self-repair in speech , 1983, Cognition.

[46]  K. Bock An Effect of the Accessibility of Word Forms on Sentence Structures , 1987 .

[47]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production : Picture word interference studies , 1990 .

[48]  W. Levelt,et al.  Normal and deviant lexical processing: Reply to Dell and O'Seaghdha (1991). , 1991 .

[49]  J. Morton,et al.  The effects of priming on picture recognition. , 1982, British journal of psychology.

[50]  Gary S. Dell,et al.  The retrieval of phonological forms in production: tests of predictions from a connectionist model , 1988 .

[51]  K Rayner,et al.  Stages of processing in word identification. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[52]  Gary S. Dell,et al.  Stages in sentence production: An analysis of speech error data , 1981 .

[53]  W. Heij,et al.  Categorical interference and associative priming in picture naming. , 1990 .

[54]  Wido La Heij,et al.  Semantic interference and orthographic facilitation in definition naming. , 1993 .

[55]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Models of visual word recognition: Sampling the state of the art. , 1994 .

[56]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  Interaction between semantic and orthographic factors in conceptually driven naming: Comment on Starreveld and La Heij (1995) , 1996 .

[57]  Anne Cutler,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[58]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Lemma retrieval in speaking: a theory, computer simulations, and empirical data , 1992 .

[59]  J. Raaijmakers,et al.  Does pizza prime coin? Perceptual priming in lexical decision and pronunciation. , 1998 .

[60]  A Caramazza,et al.  Retrieval of lexical-syntactic features in tip-of-the-tongue states. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[61]  Experimental note: Cross-modal, visual-auditory picture-word interference , 1990 .

[62]  W. Marslen-Wilson Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition , 1987, Cognition.

[63]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Perseverant responding in speeded naming of pictures: It's in the links. , 1991 .

[64]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Conceptual combinations and relational contexts in free association and in priming in lexical decision and naming , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[65]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Micro Experimental Laboratory: An integrated system for IBM PC compatibles , 1988 .

[66]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Lexical access in speech production , 1989 .

[67]  P. Briggs,et al.  The development of word recognition processes. , 1984, British journal of psychology.

[68]  G. Humphreys,et al.  An interactive activation approach to object processing: effects of structural similarity, name frequency, and task in normality and pathology. , 1995, Memory.

[69]  P. Amrhein Temporal invariance for picture-word translation: Evidence from drawing-writing and naming-reading tasks , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[70]  J. Jescheniak,et al.  Discrete serial versus cascaded processing in lexical access in speech production : Further evidence from the coactivation of near-synonyms , 1998 .

[71]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[72]  Richard R. Rosinski,et al.  Picture-word interference is semantically based. , 1977 .

[73]  G. E. MacKinnon,et al.  Reading Research Advances in Theory and Practice , 1985 .

[74]  A. Meyer Lexical Access in Phrase and Sentence Production: Results from Picture–Word Interference Experiments , 1996 .

[75]  A. Caramazza How many levels of processing are there in lexical access , 1997 .

[76]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Cascade processes in picture identification , 1988 .

[77]  Padraig G. O'Seaghdha,et al.  Mediated Semantic-Phonological Priming: Calling Distant Relatives , 1997 .

[78]  Stephen J. Lupker,et al.  Rhyme Priming of Pictures and Words: A Lexical Activation Account , 1989 .

[79]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .

[80]  W. Glaser,et al.  Context effects in stroop-like word and picture processing. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[81]  J G Snodgrass,et al.  Picture naming by young children: norms for name agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[82]  Geoffrey Underwood,et al.  SEMANTIC INTERFERENCE FROM UNATTENDED PRINTED WORDS , 1976 .

[83]  D. Norris Shortlist: a connectionist model of continuous speech recognition , 1994, Cognition.

[84]  Wido La Heij,et al.  Semantic interference, orthographic facilitation, and their interaction in naming tasks. , 1995 .

[85]  Gerry Altmann,et al.  Cognitive Models of Speech Processing: Psycholinguistic and Computational Perspectives - Workshop Overview , 1989, AI Mag..

[86]  W. Levelt,et al.  Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form , 1994 .