The role of inhibition in a spreading-activation model of language production. II. The simulational perspective

This is the second part of a project aimed at establishing the role of lateral inhibition in spreading-activation models of language production. While the first part (Berg & Schade, 1992) reviewed the psycholinguistic evidence for inhibition and established that indeed inhibition takes place, this paper addresses the issue from the implementational perspective. A series of computer simulations are reported in which spreading-activation models with or without inhibition are directly contrasted. The performance of the competing models is evaluated against two sets of criteria. On the more technical side, the “heat death” and the selection problems have to be solved. That is, the target must have a high and the nontarget units a low level of activation. Additionally, there are certain empirical effects from the production literature which may serve as test cases for deciding between the rival models. The results of the simulations indicate that only the inhibition-based model proves capable of meeting all challenges. All inhibitionless variants fail in one or more respects. It is further shown that the need for the inhibitory component increases with the size of the processing network and that one well-known production model can do without lateral inhibition only at the expense of introducing a concept which resembles inhibition in its effect but cannot replace it.

[1]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The TRACE model of speech perception , 1986, Cognitive Psychology.

[2]  A. Meyer The time course of phonological encoding in language production: Phonological encoding inside a syllable , 1991 .

[3]  Gary S. Dell,et al.  Positive Feedback in Hierarchical Connectionist Models: Applications to Language Production , 1988, Cogn. Sci..

[4]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. , 1982, Psychological review.

[5]  T. Berg The case against accommodation: Evidence from German speech error data , 1987 .

[6]  Alan Garnham,et al.  Slips of the tongue in the London-Lund corpus of spontaneous conversation , 1981 .

[7]  P. Johnson-Laird The computer and the mind: an introduction to cognitive science , 1988 .

[8]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .

[9]  Gary S. Dell,et al.  The retrieval of phonological forms in production: tests of predictions from a connectionist model , 1988 .

[10]  T. Berg,et al.  The role of inhibition in a spreading-activation model of language production. I. The psycholinguistic perspective , 1992, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research.

[11]  A. Healy,et al.  The roles of phoneme frequency, similarity, and availability in the experimental elicitation of speech errors , 1985 .

[12]  Ulrich Schade Konnektionismus: zur Modellierung der Sprachproduktion , 1992 .

[13]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[14]  Р.К. Потапова B. Thomas. Die Abbildung des Sprachproduktionsprozesses in einem Aktivationsflußmodell. Niemeyer Verlag, Tübingen, 1988 , 1989 .

[15]  Jordan B. Pollack,et al.  Massively Parallel Parsing: A Strongly Interactive Model of Natural Language Interpretation , 1988, Cogn. Sci..

[16]  Michel Bidoit,et al.  SPOKE: an object-oriented programming environment , 1991 .

[17]  Stefanie Shattuck-Hufnagel,et al.  The role of word-onset consonants in speech production planning: New evidence from speech error patterns. , 1987 .

[18]  Thomas Berg,et al.  Die Abbildung des Sprachproduktionsprozesses in einem Aktivationsflußmodell: Untersuchungen an deutschen und englischen Versprechern , 1988 .

[19]  Joseph Paul Stemberger,et al.  The lexicon in a model of language production , 1985 .

[20]  D. Mackay The Organization of Perception and Action , 1987 .

[21]  José E. García-Albea,et al.  On the autonomy of phonological encoding: Evidence from slips of the tongue in Spanish , 1991 .

[22]  Jerome A. Feldman,et al.  Connectionist Models and Their Properties , 1982, Cogn. Sci..

[23]  Gary S. Dell,et al.  Stages in sentence production: An analysis of speech error data , 1981 .

[24]  Geoffrey E. Hinton Mapping Part-Whole Hierarchies into Connectionist Networks , 1990, Artif. Intell..

[25]  G S Dell,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.