Multisensory fusion and the stochastic structure of postural sway

Abstract. We analyze the stochastic structure of postural sway and demonstrate that this structure imposes important constraints on models of postural control. Linear stochastic models of various orders were fit to the center-of-mass trajectories of subjects during quiet stance in four sensory conditions: (i) light touch and vision, (ii) light touch, (iii) vision, and (iv) neither touch nor vision. For each subject and condition, the model of appropriate order was determined, and this model was characterized by the eigenvalues and coefficients of its autocovariance function. In most cases, postural-sway trajectories were similar to those produced by a third-order model with eigenvalues corresponding to a slow first-order decay plus a faster-decaying damped oscillation. The slow-decay fraction, which we define as the slow-decay autocovariance coefficient divided by the total variance, was usually near 1. We compare the stochastic structure of our data to two linear control-theory models: (i) a proportional–integral–derivative control model in which the postural system's state is assumed to be known, and (ii) an optimal-control model in which the system's state is estimated based on noisy multisensory information using a Kalman filter. Under certain assumptions, both models have eigenvalues consistent with our results. However, the slow-decay fraction predicted by both models is less than we observe. We show that our results are more consistent with a modification of the optimal-control model in which noise is added to the computations performed by the state estimator. This modified model has a slow-decay fraction near 1 in a parameter regime in which sensory information related to the body's velocity is more accurate than sensory information related to position and acceleration. These findings suggest that: (i) computation noise is responsible for much of the variance observed in postural sway, and (ii) the postural control system under the conditions tested resides in the regime of accurate velocity information.

[1]  David A. Winter,et al.  Biomechanics and Motor Control of Human Movement , 1990 .

[2]  G E Stelmach,et al.  Postural sway characteristics of the elderly under normal and altered visual and support surface conditions. , 1991, Journal of gerontology.

[3]  H. Weinert,et al.  Bryson, A. E./ Ho, Y.-C., Applied Optimal Control, Optimization, Estimation, and Control. New York-London-Sydney-Toronto. John Wiley & Sons. 1975. 481 S., £10.90 , 1979 .

[4]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Dynamic role of vision in the control of posture in man , 1979, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  C. C. A. M. Gielen,et al.  Postural adjustments induced by simulated motion of differently structured environments , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  V. Dietz Human neuronal control of automatic functional movements: interaction between central programs and afferent input. , 1992, Physiological reviews.

[7]  G. Schöner,et al.  Position and velocity coupling of postural sway to somatosensory drive. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  David N. Lee Visual proprioceptive control of stance , 1975 .

[9]  R. Peterka,et al.  Role of somatosensory and vestibular cues in attenuating visually induced human postural sway , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  V. Dietz,et al.  Characteristics of postural instability induced by ischemic blocking of leg afferents , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  B. Guschlbauer,et al.  The significance of proprioception on postural stabilization as assessed by ischemia , 1984, Brain Research.

[12]  V. Gusev,et al.  A model for optimal processing of multisensory information in the system for maintaining body orientation in the human , 1992, Biological Cybernetics.

[13]  Herman van der Kooij,et al.  A multisensory integration model of human stance control , 1999, Biological Cybernetics.

[14]  L. Arnold Stochastic Differential Equations: Theory and Applications , 1992 .

[15]  T. Lai Time series analysis univariate and multivariate methods , 1991 .

[16]  Robert J. Peterka,et al.  Postural control model interpretation of stabilogram diffusion analysis , 2000, Biological Cybernetics.

[17]  F. Horak,et al.  Components of postural dyscontrol in the elderly: A review , 1989, Neurobiology of Aging.

[18]  V. J. Wilson,et al.  Mammalian Vestibular Physiology , 1979, Springer US.

[19]  Thomas Rosemeier,et al.  Interaction of vestibular, somatosensory and visual signals for postural control and motion perception under terrestrial and microgravity conditions—a conceptual model , 1998, Brain Research Reviews.

[20]  Martin A. Giese,et al.  Frequency dependence of the action-perception cycle for postural control in a moving visual environment: relative phase dynamics , 1994, Biological Cybernetics.

[21]  P. Riley,et al.  Double-Blind, Placebo-Controlled Trial of Rehabilitation for Bilateral Vestibular Hypofunction: Preliminary Report , 1993, Otolaryngology--head and neck surgery : official journal of American Academy of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery.

[22]  Robert F. Stengel,et al.  Optimal Control and Estimation , 1994 .

[23]  John E. Dennis,et al.  Numerical methods for unconstrained optimization and nonlinear equations , 1983, Prentice Hall series in computational mathematics.

[24]  C. Ansley An algorithm for the exact likelihood of a mixed autoregressive-moving average process , 1979 .

[25]  Raymond M. Redheffer,et al.  Differential Equations: Theory And Applications , 1991 .

[26]  Ian P. Howard,et al.  Human visual orientation , 1982 .

[27]  J. Lackner,et al.  Coupling of fingertip somatosensory information to head and body sway , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  Tim Kiemel,et al.  Multisensory information for human postural control: integrating touch and vision , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  George M. Siouris,et al.  Applied Optimal Control: Optimization, Estimation, and Control , 1979, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.

[30]  Frans C. T. van der Helm,et al.  An adaptive model of sensory integration in a dynamic environment applied to human stance control , 2001, Biological Cybernetics.

[31]  J. Collins,et al.  Open-loop and closed-loop control of posture: A random-walk analysis of center-of-pressure trajectories , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  G. Schöner Dynamic theory of action-perception patterns: the “moving room” paradigm , 1991, Biological Cybernetics.

[33]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  The role of vision in the control of posture during linear motion. , 1979, Progress in brain research.

[34]  William W. S. Wei,et al.  Time series analysis - univariate and multivariate methods , 1989 .

[35]  A.D. Kuo,et al.  An optimal control model for analyzing human postural balance , 1995, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[36]  L. Nashner Analysis of Stance Posture in Humans , 1981 .

[37]  Måns Magnusson,et al.  Identification of human postural dynamics , 1988 .