Developmental contributions to motor sequence learning

Little is known about how children acquire new motor sequences. In particular, it is not clear if the same learning progression observed in adults is also present in childhood nor whether motor skills are acquired in a similar fashion across development. In the present study we used the multi-finger sequencing task (MFST), a variant of the serial reaction time (SRT) task, to study motor sequence learning, across two consecutive days, in three cross-sectional samples of children aged 6, 8, and 10 years, and a control sample of adults. In the MFST, participants reproduced 10-element sequences of key presses on an electronic keyboard, using four fingers of the right hand. Each block of practice included 10 intermixed trials of a Repeated (REP) sequence and four trials of Random (RAN) sequences. Performance was assessed by examining changes in accuracy, a component of the task that requires the association of the visual stimulus with the motor response, and response synchronization, a component that requires fine-grained sensorimotor integration and timing. Additionally, participants completed Recognition and Recall tests, to assess explicit knowledge of the repeated sequence. Overall, results showed a developmental progression in motor sequence learning within and across days of practice. Interestingly, the two behavioral measures showed different developmental trajectories. For accuracy, differences were greatest for the two youngest groups early in learning, and these groups also showed the greatest rate of improvement. However, by the end of Day 2, only the 6-year-olds still lagged behind all other groups. For response synchronization, all child groups differed from adults early in learning, but both child and adult groups showed similar rates of improvement across blocks of practice. By the end of Day 2, 10-year-olds reached adult levels of performance, whereas 6- and 8-year-olds did not. Taken together, the dissociation observed with our two behavioral measures of sequence learning is consistent with the hypothesis that accuracy or finger-stimulus association may rely on cortical pathways that show the greatest maturation between ages 6 and 10; whereas motor timing and sensorimotor integration may rely on subcortical pathways that continue to develop into young adulthood. Despite developmental differences across blocks of practice on both behavioral measures, there were no significant group differences for either the Recognition or Recall tests. We suggest that explicit knowledge of the MFST is not directly linked to task performance, thus challenging the implicit–explicit distinction in pediatric SRT studies assessing the developmental invariance model.

[1]  G. O'driscoll,et al.  Executive Functions and Methylphenidate Response in Subtypes of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[2]  Avi Karni,et al.  Reduced Susceptibility to Interference in the Consolidation of Motor Memory before Adolescence , 2007, PloS one.

[3]  G. Schlaug The Brain of Musicians , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[4]  M. Denckla Development of Speed in Repetitive and Successive Finger‐movements in Normal Children , 1973, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[5]  J. Piek,et al.  Fine and gross motor ability in males with ADHD , 2003, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[6]  Michelle Fleury,et al.  Coordination in childhood: modifications of visuomotor representations in 6- to 11-year-old children , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  J. Born,et al.  Sleep forms memory for finger skills , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[8]  Jane E. Clark,et al.  Development of visuomotor representations for hand movement in young children , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  M. Badan,et al.  Sequential pointing in children and adults. , 2000, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[10]  J. S. Johnson,et al.  Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[11]  M. Lassonde,et al.  Callosal Contribution to Procedural Learning in Children , 2001, Developmental Neuropsychology.

[12]  D. Shanks IMPLICIT LEARNING AND TACIT KNOWLEDGE - AN ESSAY ON THE COGNITIVE UNCONSCIOUS - REBER,A , 1995 .

[13]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Current concepts in procedural consolidation , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[14]  Tal Savion-Lemieux,et al.  The effect of early musical training on adult motor performance: evidence for a sensitive period in motor learning , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Sleep and the time course of motor skill learning. , 2003, Learning & memory.

[16]  D. Dewey,et al.  Motor Correlates of ADHD , 2005, Journal of learning disabilities.

[17]  K. Reilly,et al.  Independence of force production by digits of the human hand , 2000, Neuroscience Letters.

[18]  Tal Savion-Lemieux,et al.  The effects of practice and delay on motor skill learning and retention , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Consolidation of motor memory , 2006, Trends in Neurosciences.

[20]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  Norman M. Weinberger,et al.  Consequences of failures to meet standards in learning and memory , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[22]  V. Menon,et al.  White matter development during childhood and adolescence: a cross-sectional diffusion tensor imaging study. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  J. Bartko,et al.  Cortical correlates of neuromotor development in healthy children , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[24]  Suzanne E. Welcome,et al.  Longitudinal Mapping of Cortical Thickness and Brain Growth in Normal Children , 2022 .

[25]  Dan Nemet,et al.  Neuromotor noise limits motor performance, but not motor adaptation, in children. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  E. Knudsen Sensitive Periods in the Development of the Brain and Behavior , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  Philip Shaw,et al.  Cerebellar development and clinical outcome in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. , 2007, The American journal of psychiatry.

[28]  M. Illert,et al.  Development of prehension movements in children: a kinematic study , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  Thomas F. Nugent,et al.  Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[30]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Structural maturation of neural pathways in children and adolescents: in vivo study. , 1999, Science.

[31]  M. Van der Linden,et al.  Implicit sequence learning in children. , 1998, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[32]  Kae Nakamura,et al.  Central mechanisms of motor skill learning , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[33]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Jacques Duysens,et al.  Developmental trends in speed accuracy trade-off in 6-10-year-old children performing rapid reciprocal and discrete aiming movements. , 2006, Human movement science.

[35]  M. Nissen,et al.  Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[36]  J. Doyon,et al.  Reorganization and plasticity in the adult brain during learning of motor skills , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[37]  P. Wolff,et al.  ASSOCIATED MOVEMENTS AS A MEASURE OF DEVELOPMENTAL AGE , 1983, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[38]  Tamar Flash,et al.  Multiple shifts in the representation of a motor sequence during the acquisition of skilled performance , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[39]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Two Forms of Sequential Implicit Learning , 1997, Consciousness and Cognition.

[40]  Ingeborg Krägeloh-Mann,et al.  Global and local development of gray and white matter volume in normal children and adolescents , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  H. Neville,et al.  Sensitive periods differentiate processing of open- and closed-class words: an ERP study of bilinguals. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[42]  M H Schieber,et al.  Quantifying the Independence of Human Finger Movements: Comparisons of Digits, Hands, and Movement Frequencies , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  S. Slobounov,et al.  Modulated cortical control of individual fingers in experienced musicians: an EEG study , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[44]  K. Reilly,et al.  Intrinsic hand muscles and digit independence on the preferred and non-preferred hands of humans , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[45]  A. Toga,et al.  In vivo evidence for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  K. Doya,et al.  Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[47]  Stefan Fischer,et al.  Developmental Differences in Sleep's Role for Implicit Off-line Learning: Comparing Children with Adults , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Maturation of white matter in the human brain: a review of magnetic resonance studies , 2001, Brain Research Bulletin.

[49]  C. Nelson,et al.  Serial reaction time learning in preschool- and school-age children. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[50]  Marc H Schieber,et al.  Human finger independence: limitations due to passive mechanical coupling versus active neuromuscular control. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  Tobias Sommer,et al.  Evidence of Developmental Differences in Implicit Sequence Learning: An fMRI Study of Children and Adults , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.