The “Law of Brevity” in animal communication: Sex‐specific signaling optimization is determined by call amplitude rather than duration
暂无分享,去创建一个
Amiyaal Ilany | Vlad Demartsev | A. Barocas | E. Geffen | Vlad Demartsev | A. Ilany | Einat Bar-Ziv | Eli Geffen | Adi Barocas | Tchia Ilany | Yael Goll | Naomi Gordon | Einat Bar-Ziv | Yael Goll | Tchia Ilany | Naomi Gordon
[1] J. Hailman,et al. The ‘chick-a-dee’ calls of Parus atricapillus: A recombinant system of animal communication compared with written English , 1985 .
[2] Ramon Ferrer-i-Cancho,et al. Testing the Robustness of Laws of Polysemy and Brevity Versus Frequency , 2016, SLSP.
[3] P. B. Fourie. Acoustic Communication in the Rock Hyrax, Procavia capensis , 1977 .
[4] Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al. Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[5] Gábor Győri. Animal communication and human language , 1995 .
[6] E. Geffen,et al. Individual identity is communicated through multiple pathways in male rock hyrax (Procavia capensis) songs , 2011, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[7] A. Barocas,et al. The progression pattern of male hyrax songs and the role of climactic ending , 2017, Scientific Reports.
[8] W. Getz,et al. Hierarchical dominance structure and social organization in African elephants, Loxodonta africana , 2007, Animal Behaviour.
[9] Amiyaal Ilany,et al. Variance in Centrality within Rock Hyrax Social Networks Predicts Adult Longevity , 2011, PloS one.
[10] Michael Tomasello,et al. Gestural communication in young gorillas (Gorilla gorilla): Gestural repertoire, learning, and use , 2003, American journal of primatology.
[11] S. Pika,et al. Referential gestural communication in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 2006, Current Biology.
[12] K. Hammerschmidt,et al. Responses of Redfronted Lemurs to Experimentally Modified Alarm Calls: Evidence for Urgency-Based Changes in Call Structure , 2002 .
[13] P. Slater,et al. The different roles of social learning in vocal communication , 2000, Animal Behaviour.
[14] Joseph A. Scott. Comfort and Seating Distance in Living Rooms , 1984 .
[15] W. Fitch,et al. Vertebrate Vocal Production: An Introductory Overview , 2016 .
[16] U. Jürgens,et al. The neural control of vocalization in mammals: a review. , 2009, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.
[17] Alban Lemasson,et al. Sex differences in the vocal repertoire of adult red‐capped mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus): a multi‐level acoustic analysis , 2010, American journal of primatology.
[18] Jiang Feng,et al. Brevity is prevalent in bat short-range communication , 2013, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[19] Kobin H. Kendrick,et al. Taking turns: bridging the gap between human and animal communication , 2018, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[20] Yuen Ren Chao,et al. Human Behavior and the Principle of Least Effort: An Introduction to Human Ecology , 1950 .
[21] A. Barocas,et al. Male hyraxes increase song complexity and duration in the presence of alert individuals , 2014 .
[22] Andrey Anikin,et al. Human Non-linguistic Vocal Repertoire: Call Types and Their Meaning , 2017, Journal of Nonverbal Behavior.
[23] Marc Naguib,et al. Estimating the distance to a source of sound: mechanisms and adaptations for long-range communication , 2001, Animal Behaviour.
[24] A. Barocas,et al. A mixed strategy of counter-singing behavior in male rock hyrax vocal competitions , 2016, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[25] Todd M Freeberg,et al. Information theoretical approaches to chick-a-dee calls of Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis). , 2012, Journal of comparative psychology.
[26] I. Titze,et al. Radiation efficiency for long-range vocal communication in mammals and birds. , 2018, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[27] E. Geffen,et al. Social status and cortisol levels in singing rock hyraxes , 2008, Hormones and Behavior.
[28] Ramon Ferrer-i-Cancho,et al. The Failure of the Law of Brevity in Two New World Primates. Statistical Caveats. , 2012, ArXiv.
[29] Robin C. Dunkin,et al. The metabolic cost of communicative sound production in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) , 2013, Journal of Experimental Biology.
[30] E. Geffen,et al. Elevated testosterone levels and social ranks in female rock hyrax , 2006, Hormones and Behavior.
[31] H. Hoeck. Demography and competition in Hyrax , 1989, Oecologia.
[32] J. M. Jenkins,et al. The appropriate use of Zipf's law in animal communication studies , 2005, Animal Behaviour.
[33] David Lusseau,et al. Compression as a Universal Principle of Animal Behavior , 2013, Cogn. Sci..
[34] Govindasamy Agoramoorthy,et al. Efficiency of coding in macaque vocal communication , 2010, Biology Letters.
[35] D. Reby,et al. The descended larynx is not uniquely human , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[36] Sven Grawunder,et al. Reducing language to rhythm: Amazonian Bora drummed language exploits speech rhythm for long-distance communication , 2018, Royal Society Open Science.
[37] Amiyaal Ilany,et al. The energy cost of singing in wild rock hyrax males: evidence for an index signal , 2013, Animal Behaviour.
[38] Marla M. Holt,et al. Vocal performance affects metabolic rate in dolphins: implications for animals communicating in noisy environments , 2015, The Journal of Experimental Biology.
[39] P. Laiolo,et al. Information theory reveals that individual birds do not alter song complexity when varying song length , 2014, Animal Behaviour.
[40] A. Radford,et al. Brevity is not always a virtue in primate communication , 2011, Biology Letters.
[41] E. Geffen,et al. Complex call in male rock hyrax (Procavia capensis): a multi-information distributing channel , 2009, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[42] J. M. Pickett,et al. Effects of Vocal Force on the Intelligibility of Speech Sounds , 1956 .
[43] Ricard V. Solé,et al. Least effort and the origins of scaling in human language , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[44] G. Zipf. The meaning-frequency relationship of words. , 1945, The Journal of general psychology.
[45] A. Lemasson,et al. Social complexity parallels vocal complexity: a comparison of three non-human primate species , 2013, Front. Psychol..
[46] Ramon Ferrer-i-Cancho,et al. Acoustic sequences in non‐human animals: a tutorial review and prospectus , 2016, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.
[47] F Goller,et al. The metabolic cost of birdsong production. , 2001, The Journal of experimental biology.
[48] Amiyaal Ilany,et al. Do Singing Rock Hyraxes Exploit Conspecific Calls to Gain Attention? , 2011, PloS one.
[49] Hinrich Schütze,et al. Book Reviews: Foundations of Statistical Natural Language Processing , 1999, CL.
[50] Kimberly A. Pollard,et al. Evolving communicative complexity: insights from rodents and beyond , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[51] K. Zuberbühler,et al. Diana monkey long-distance calls: messages for conspecifics and predators , 1997, Animal Behaviour.
[52] K. Zuberbühler,et al. Graded or discrete? A quantitative analysis of Campbell's monkey alarm calls , 2013, Animal Behaviour.
[53] A. Barocas,et al. Individual, social, and sexual niche traits affect copulation success in a polygynandrous mating system , 2016, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[54] David R. Wilson,et al. Fowl communicate the size, speed and proximity of avian stimuli through graded structure in referential alarm calls , 2012, Animal Behaviour.
[55] K. Gwet. Computing inter-rater reliability and its variance in the presence of high agreement. , 2008, The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology.
[56] Morgan L. Gustison,et al. A survey of the context and structure of high- and low-amplitude calls in mammals , 2015, Animal Behaviour.
[57] LAURANCE R. DOYLE,et al. Quantitative tools for comparing animal communication systems: information theory applied to bottlenose dolphin whistle repertoires , 1999, Animal Behaviour.
[58] Vlad Demartsev,et al. Harsh vocal elements affect counter-singing dynamics in male rock hyrax , 2016 .
[59] R. Howard. Principles of Animal Communication , 1999 .
[60] David Lusseau,et al. Efficient coding in dolphin surface behavioral patterns , 2009, Complex..
[61] W. Fitch,et al. The Biology and Evolution of Speech: A Comparative Analysis , 2018 .
[62] Ramon Ferrer-i-Cancho,et al. Linguistic laws in chimpanzee gestural communication , 2019, Proceedings of the Royal Society B.
[63] L. Mech,et al. WOLF HOWLING AND ITS ROLE IN TERRITORY MAINTENANCE , 1979 .
[64] Thierry Aubin,et al. Are bird song complexity and song sharing shaped by habitat structure? An information theory and statistical approach. , 2010, Journal of theoretical biology.
[65] B. McCowan,et al. Amplitude of bison bellows reflects male quality, physical condition and motivation , 2008, Animal Behaviour.