Effects of mental and physical demands on heart rate variability during computer work

Abstract. The aim of the study was to evaluate the effect of physical and mental demands on heart rate variability- (HRV-) derived indices of autonomic activity. Ten healthy, female subjects performed two computer tasks: one with combined mental and physical demands and a reference task primarily consisting of physical demands. The combined task, which was performed once with a keyboard and once with a computer mouse, was a computerized version of the colour word conflict task (CWT). The CWT is highly mentally demanding due to the inherent perceptual conflict between a word stimulus and a colour stimulus. In the reference task (REF) the physical demands were comparable to CWT, while the mental demands were low. Finally, the subjects rested at the workplace (REST). Data on performance, heart rate (HR), mean arterial blood pressure (MAP), HRV, and urinary concentrations of catecholamines were obtained. The following frequency bands were applied for HRV: very low frequency (VLF, 0.00–0.04 Hz), low frequency (LF, 0.05–0.15 Hz), high frequency (HF, 0.16–0.40 Hz) and total power (TP, 0.00–0.40 Hz). Indices of sympathetic nervous activity (ISNS) and parasympathetic nervous activity (IPNS) were estimated as normalized powers in LF and HF bands: ISNS=LF/(TP–VLF) and IPNS=HF/(TP–VLF). Values are expressed as normalised units (nu). There was an increase in ISNS during CWT [mouse: 0.490 (0.052) nu [ave (SEM)] and keyboard: 0.476 (0.039) nu] and REF [mouse: 0.453 (0.059) nu and keyboard: 0.489 (0.047) nu] compared to REST [0.397 (0.047) nu], but no difference between CWT and REF. Corresponding decreases were observed for IPNS. HR and MAP were higher during CWT compared to REST. No effects were observed for excreted amounts of catecholamines. There were no differences between the computer mouse and the keyboard condition for ISNS and IPNS. In conclusion, an increase in ISNS and a decrease in IPNS were found in response to a physically demanding reference computer task. Addition of mental demands did not elicit any further effect on ISNS and IPNS, suggesting a significant influence of the physical rather than the mental demands during computer work.

[1]  H. Warner,et al.  A Mathematical Model of Heart Rate Control by Sympathetic and Vagus Efferent Information , 1962, Journal of applied physiology.

[2]  P Hjemdahl,et al.  Differentiated sympathetic activation during mental stress evoked by the Stroop test. , 1984, Acta physiologica Scandinavica. Supplementum.

[3]  D. Adam,et al.  Assessment of autonomic function in humans by heart rate spectral analysis. , 1985, The American journal of physiology.

[4]  R. Cohen,et al.  Hemodynamic regulation: investigation by spectral analysis. , 1985, The American journal of physiology.

[5]  M. Turiel,et al.  Power Spectral Analysis of Heart Rate and Arterial Pressure Variabilities as a Marker of Sympatho‐Vagal Interaction in Man and Conscious Dog , 1986, Circulation research.

[6]  J. Tulen,et al.  Characterization of stress reactions to the Stroop Color Word Test , 1989, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[7]  R. Hughson,et al.  Autonomic control of heart rate during exercise studied by heart rate variability spectral analysis. , 1991, Journal of applied physiology.

[8]  S Cerutti,et al.  Sympathovagal interaction during mental stress. A study using spectral analysis of heart rate variability in healthy control subjects and patients with a prior myocardial infarction. , 1991, Circulation.

[9]  P. Jorna Spectral analysis of heart rate and psychological state: A review of its validity as a workload index , 1992, Biological Psychology.

[10]  L. Mulder Measurement and analysis methods of heart rate and respiration for use in applied environments , 1992, Biological Psychology.

[11]  K. Madden,et al.  Effects of mental state on heart rate and blood pressure variability in men and women. , 1995, Clinical physiology.

[12]  A. Malliani,et al.  Heart rate variability. Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use , 1996 .

[13]  Y. Yamamoto,et al.  Effects of Stroop color-word conflict test on the autonomic nervous system responses. , 1997, The American journal of physiology.

[14]  S. Miyake,et al.  Power spectral analysis of heart rate variability in type A females during a psychomotor task. , 1998, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[15]  J. Fortrat,et al.  White coat effect and reactivity to stress: cardiovascular and autonomic nervous system responses. , 1998, Hypertension.

[16]  J. M. Christensen,et al.  Validation of a high performance liquid chromatography analysis for the determination of noradrenaline and adrenaline in human urine with an on-line sample purification. , 1999, Talanta.

[17]  P. Sleight,et al.  Effects of controlled breathing, mental activity and mental stress with or without verbalization on heart rate variability. , 2000, Journal of the American College of Cardiology.

[18]  Arsenio Veicsteinas,et al.  Heart rate variability during dynamic exercise in elderly males and females , 2000, European Journal of Applied Physiology.

[19]  R. González-Camarena,et al.  Effect of static and dynamic exercise on heart rate and blood pressure variabilities. , 2000, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[20]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[21]  Influence of head-down and lateral decubitus neck flexion on heart rate variability. , 2001, Journal of applied physiology.

[22]  Jens Wahlström,et al.  Physical load in computer mouse work Working technique, sex and stress aspects , 2001 .