Domain specificity in conceptual development: Neuropsychological evidence from autism

To understand some aspects of conceptual development it is necessary to take cognitive architecture into account. For this purpose, the study of normal development is often not sufficient. Fortunately, one can also study neurodevelopmental disorders. For example, autistic children have severe difficulties developing certain kinds of concepts but not others. We find that whereas autistic children perform very poorly on tests of the concept, believes, they are at or near ceiling on comparable tasks that test understanding of pictorial representation. A similar pattern was found in a second study which looked at understanding of a false map or diagram: normal 4-year-olds showed a marked advantage in understanding a false belief over a false map, while the autistic subjects performed better on the map. These findings suggest that the concept, believes, develops as a domain-specific notion that is not equatable with "having a picture (map or diagram) in the head." This result supports the existence of a specialized cognitive mechanism, which subserves the development of folk psychological notions, and which is dissociably damaged in autism. We extend these ideas to outline a new model of the development of false belief performance.

[1]  H. Wellman,et al.  Young children's attribution of action to beliefs and desires. , 1989 .

[2]  M. Rutter Language Disorder and Infantile Autism , 1978 .

[3]  J. Perner,et al.  Does the autistic child have a metarepresentational deficit? , 1991, Cognition.

[4]  U. Frith,et al.  Prospects for a cognitive neuropsychology of autism: Hobson's choice. , 1990, Psychological review.

[5]  Alan M. Leslie,et al.  Autism and ostensive communication: The relevance of metarepresentation , 1989, Development and Psychopathology.

[6]  James A. Russell,et al.  The ‘windows task’ as a measure of strategic deception in preschoolers and autistic subjects , 1991 .

[7]  J. Morton,et al.  The cognitive basis of a biological disorder: autism , 1991, Trends in Neurosciences.

[8]  S. Wulff,et al.  The symbolic and object play of children with autism: A review , 1985, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[9]  M. Farah The neurological basis of mental imagery: A componential analysis , 1984, Cognition.

[10]  Keith A. Coleman,et al.  On the Propositional Attitudes , 1991 .

[11]  U. Frith,et al.  A new look at language and communication in autism. , 1989, The British journal of disorders of communication.

[12]  Richard J. Brown Neuropsychology Mental Structure , 1989 .

[13]  S. Baron-Cohen Are autistic children “Behaviorists”? An examination of their mental-physical and appearance-reality distinctions , 1989, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[14]  M. Prior,et al.  Autistic children's knowledge of thinking and feeling states in other people. , 1990, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[15]  B. Pennington,et al.  Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. , 1991, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[16]  D. Zaitchik Is only seeing really believing?: Sources of the true belief in the false belief task , 1991 .

[17]  P. Mundy,et al.  Social interactions of autistic, mentally retarded and normal children and their caregivers. , 1986, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[18]  A. Leslie,et al.  Autistic children's understanding of seeing, knowing and believing , 1988 .

[19]  J. Perner Understanding the Representational Mind , 1993 .

[20]  P. Mitchell,et al.  Children's early understanding of false belief , 1991, Cognition.

[21]  Alison Gopnik,et al.  The ontogeny of common sense. , 1988 .

[22]  N. Cocchiarella,et al.  Situations and Attitudes. , 1986 .

[23]  B. Bower A Child's Theory of Mind , 1993 .

[24]  Alison Gopnik,et al.  Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. , 1988 .

[25]  U. Frith Autism: Explaining the Enigma , 2005 .

[26]  S Baron-Cohen,et al.  The development of a theory of mind in autism: deviance and delay? , 1991, The Psychiatric clinics of North America.

[27]  J. Boucher,et al.  The theory of mind hypothesis of autism: explanation, evidence and assessment. , 1989, The British journal of disorders of communication.

[28]  Alan M. Leslie,et al.  Pretense, Autism, and the Theory-of-Mind Module , 1992 .

[29]  D. Zaitchik,et al.  When representations conflict with reality: The preschooler's problem with false beliefs and “false” photographs , 1990, Cognition.

[30]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Against the Cartesian view on mind: Young children's difficulty with own false beliefs , 1991 .

[31]  Simon Baron-Cohen,et al.  Perceptual role taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism , 1989 .

[32]  Henry M. Wellman,et al.  Young children's reasoning about beliefs , 1988, Cognition.

[33]  A. Leslie,et al.  Exploration of the autistic child's theory of mind: knowledge, belief, and communication. , 1989, Child development.

[34]  M. Sigman,et al.  Symbolic play and language comprehension in autistic children. , 1981, Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry.

[35]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Young Children's Understanding of Changes in Their Mental States. , 1991 .

[36]  John H. Flavell,et al.  The development of children's knowledge about the mind: From cognitive connections to mental representations. , 1988 .

[37]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Understanding drawings and beliefs: a further test of the metarepresentation theory of autism: a research note. , 1992, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[38]  A. Leslie,et al.  The recognition of attitude conveyed by utterance: A study of preschool and autistic children , 1991 .

[39]  A. Leslie Pretense and representation: The origins of "theory of mind." , 1987 .

[40]  D. Olson,et al.  Developing theories of mind , 1988 .

[41]  R. P. Hobson,et al.  On acquiring knowledge about people and the capacity to pretend: response to Leslie (1987) , 1990, Psychological review.

[42]  A. Leslie The theory of mind impairment in autism: Evidence for a modular mechanism of development? , 1991 .

[43]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. , 1988, Child development.

[44]  Uta Frith,et al.  The understanding and use of interpersonal gestures by autistic and Down's syndrome children , 1988, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[45]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Understanding other minds : perspectives from autism , 1994 .

[46]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[47]  H. Wellman The Child's Theory of Mind , 1990 .

[48]  C. Lewis,et al.  Three-year-olds' problems with false belief: conceptual deficit or linguistic artifact? , 1990, Child development.

[49]  L. Wing,et al.  Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and classification , 1979, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[50]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Mechanical, behavioural and Intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children , 1986 .

[51]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Three-year-olds' difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit , 1987 .

[52]  S. Baron-Cohen Autism and symbolic play , 1987 .

[53]  G. Butterworth Perspectives on the child's theory of mind , 1991 .

[54]  J. Fodor The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .

[55]  L. Kanner Autistic disturbances of affective contact. , 1968, Acta paedopsychiatrica.

[56]  J. Morton 4 Developmental Contingency Modelling A framework for discussing the processes of chang and the consequence of deficiency , 1986 .

[57]  A. Leslie The necessity of illusion: Perception and thought in infancy , 1988 .