Mental representation: What can pitch tell us about the distance effect?
暂无分享,去创建一个
Avishai Henik | Roi Cohen Kadosh | Warren Brodsky | A. Henik | R. C. Kadosh | W. Brodsky | Michal Levin | Michael E. Levin
[1] Wolf Schwarz,et al. Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.
[2] Jamie I. D. Campbell. Handbook of mathematical cognition , 2004 .
[3] Avishai Henik,et al. Notation-Dependent and -Independent Representations of Numbers in the Parietal Lobes , 2007, Neuron.
[4] Wim Fias,et al. Spatial representation of numbers. , 2004 .
[5] J. D. Warren,et al. Separating pitch chroma and pitch height in the human brain , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[6] F. Restle. Speed of Adding and Comparing Numbers. , 1970 .
[7] Roi Cohen Kadosh,et al. The laterality effect: Myth or truth? , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.
[8] S Dehaene,et al. Attention, automaticity, and levels of representation in number processing. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[9] Avishai Henik,et al. A common representation for semantic and physical properties: a cognitive-anatomical approach. , 2006, Experimental psychology.
[10] Avishai Henik,et al. The Neuronal Correlate of Bidirectional Synesthesia: A Combined Event-related Potential and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[11] Robert W Proctor,et al. Stimulus and response representations underlying orthogonal stimulus-response compatibility effects , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[12] I. Peretz,et al. Brain organization for music processing. , 2005, Annual review of psychology.
[13] A. Henik,et al. Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.
[14] Wim Fias,et al. The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized , 2003, Cognition.
[15] Wim Fias,et al. Automatic response activation of implicit spatial information: Evidence from the SNARC effect. , 2006, Acta psychologica.
[16] Philippe Pinel,et al. Distributed and Overlapping Cerebral Representations of Number, Size, and Luminance during Comparative Judgments , 2004, Neuron.
[17] Roi Cohen Kadosh,et al. Are numbers special? An overview of chronometric, neuroimaging, developmental and comparative studies of magnitude representation , 2008, Progress in Neurobiology.
[18] David J. Ostry,et al. Time course of number magnitude interference during grasping , 2008, Cortex.
[19] Wim Fias,et al. The Mental Representation of Ordinal Sequences is Spatially Organised: Evidence from Days of the Week , 2004, Cortex.
[20] Dana Ganor-Stern,et al. Automaticity in Processing Ordinal Information , 2004 .
[21] I. Peretz. The nature of music from a biological perspective , 2006, Cognition.
[22] Avishai Henik,et al. The neuronal correlate of bi-directional synaesthesia: A combined ERP and fMRI study , 2007 .
[23] Bruno L. Giordano,et al. Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect , 2006, Cognition.
[24] K. Ohgushi,et al. Perceptual components of pitch: spatial representation using a multidimensional scaling technique. , 1987, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[25] J. Lammertyn,et al. The hunt for SNARC , 2005 .
[26] Avishai Henik,et al. Are numbers special? The comparison systems of the human brain investigated by fMRI , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[27] A. Henik,et al. A colorful walk on the mental number line: Striving for the right direction , 2008, Cognition.
[28] Vincent Walsh. A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[29] V. Csépe,et al. The speed of magnitude processing and executive functions in controlled and automatic number comparison in children: an electro-encephalography study , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Functions.
[30] M. H. Fischer,et al. Do Negative Numbers Have a Place on the Mental Number Line , 2005 .
[31] H. Heinze,et al. On the interaction of numerical and size information in digit comparison: a behavioral and event-related potential study , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[32] Avishai Henik,et al. The Brain Locus of Interaction between Number and Size: A Combined Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Event-related Potential Study , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[33] M. Corbetta,et al. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[34] Stefan Golaszewski,et al. Neural correlates of distance and congruity effects in a numerical Stroop task: an event-related fMRI study , 2005, NeuroImage.
[35] Selecting between intelligent options , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[36] Samuel Shaki,et al. On the mental representation of negative numbers: Context-dependent SNARC effects with comparative judgments , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[37] Tom Verguts,et al. Dissociation of the distance effect and size effect in one-digit numbers , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[38] ROBERT S. MOYER,et al. Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.
[39] Raymond J. Dolan,et al. Imaging Informational Conflict: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Numerical Stroop , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[40] Avishai Henik,et al. When brightness counts: the neuronal correlate of numerical-luminance interference. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.
[41] N. Kanwisher,et al. The Generality of Parietal Involvement in Visual Attention , 1999, Neuron.
[42] Avishai Henik,et al. Basic numerical processing in left intraparietal sulcus (IPS) acalculia , 2008, Cortex.
[43] Takeshi Hatta,et al. Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect , 2004, Memory & cognition.
[44] G. Orban,et al. Parietal Representation of Symbolic and Nonsymbolic Magnitude , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[45] M. Pesenti,et al. Numerosity-duration interference: a Stroop experiment. , 2006, Acta psychologica.
[46] A. Henik,et al. A synesthetic walk on the mental number line: The size effect , 2008, Cognition.
[47] Eliot Hazeltine,et al. Dissociable Contributions of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices to Response Selection , 2002, NeuroImage.
[48] Avishai Henik,et al. Automatic and intentional processing of numerical information , 1992 .
[49] Avishai Henik,et al. Virtual Dyscalculia Induced by Parietal-Lobe TMS Impairs Automatic Magnitude Processing , 2007, Current Biology.
[50] Guilherme Wood,et al. Neural representations of two-digit numbers: A parametric fMRI study , 2006, NeuroImage.
[51] Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al. Response-Selection-Related Parietal Activation during Number Comparison , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[52] S. Dehaene,et al. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .
[53] Michael Andres,et al. Dissociation of numerosity and duration processing in the left intraparietal sulcus: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study , 2008, Cortex.
[54] Masami Ishihara,et al. Horizontal spatial representations of time: Evidence for the STEARC effect , 2008, Cortex.