Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.7 July 2005 The neural code for written words: a proposal

[1]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Subliminal Convergence of Kanji and Kana Words: Further Evidence for Functional Parcellation of the Posterior Temporal Cortex in Visual Word Perception , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  Michal Lavidor,et al.  Why word length only matters in the left visual field , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  Hidenao Fukuyama,et al.  Implicit and explicit processing of kanji and kana words and non-words studied with fMRI , 2004, NeuroImage.

[4]  Helen L Jamison,et al.  Morphology and the internal structure of words. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  Jeffrey S Bowers,et al.  What do letter migration errors reveal about letter position coding in visual word recognition? , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  Charles E Connor,et al.  Underlying principles of visual shape selectivity in posterior inferotemporal cortex , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  S. Lupker,et al.  Can CANISO activate CASINO? Transposed-letter similarity effects with nonadjacent letter positions ☆ , 2004 .

[8]  J. Grainger,et al.  Letter position coding in printed word perception: Effects of repeated and transposed letters , 2004 .

[9]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Specialization within the ventral stream: the case for the visual word form area , 2004, NeuroImage.

[10]  J B Poline,et al.  Letter Binding and Invariant Recognition of Masked Words , 2004, Psychological science.

[11]  T. Nazir,et al.  Reading habits, perceptual learning, and recognition of printed words , 2004, Brain and Language.

[12]  M. Taft,et al.  The nature of the mental representation of radicals in Chinese: a priming study. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  J. Grainger,et al.  Does the huamn mnid raed wrods as a wlohe? , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  S. Lupker,et al.  Does jugde activate COURT? Transposed-letter similarity effects in masked associative priming , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[15]  D. Willows,et al.  The Incidence and Nature of Letter Orientation Errors in Reading Disability , 2002, Journal of learning disabilities.

[16]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the Visual Word Form Area. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  R. Malach,et al.  The topography of high-order human object areas , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  Michal Lavidor,et al.  Analysis of standard and non-standard visual word format in the two hemispheres , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The priming method: imaging unconscious repetition priming reveals an abstract representation of number in the parietal lobes. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  Y. Yamane,et al.  Complex objects are represented in macaque inferotemporal cortex by the combination of feature columns , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  J B Poline,et al.  Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  C. Whitney How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[23]  Muge M. Bakircioglu,et al.  Mapping visual cortex in monkeys and humans using surface-based atlases , 2001, Vision Research.

[24]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  fMR-adaptation: a tool for studying the functional properties of human cortical neurons. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[25]  M Coltheart,et al.  DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.

[26]  T. M. Ellison,et al.  Eye-fixation behavior, lexical storage, and visual word recognition in a split processing model. , 2000, Psychological review.

[27]  E. Rolls Functions of the Primate Temporal Lobe Cortical Visual Areas in Invariant Visual Object and Face Recognition , 2000, Neuron.

[28]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Learning to find a shape , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  C R Olson,et al.  Mirror-image confusion in single neurons of the macaque inferotemporal cortex. , 2000, Science.

[30]  S Lehéricy,et al.  The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  D. Perani,et al.  A cultural effect on brain function , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  T. Poggio,et al.  Hierarchical models of object recognition in cortex , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  J Grainger,et al.  The role of letter identity and letter position in orthographic priming , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[34]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: insights from connectionist models. , 1999, Psychological review.

[35]  B. Ans,et al.  A connectionist multiple-trace memory model for polysyllabic word reading. , 1998, Psychological review.

[36]  M. Zorzi,et al.  Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. , 1998 .

[37]  J Grainger,et al.  Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: a multiple read-out model. , 1996, Psychological review.

[38]  Stuart R. Butler,et al.  The effects of visual cortex lesions on the perception of rotated shapes , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.

[39]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[40]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Psychophysical and physiological evidence for viewer-centered object representations in the primate. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[41]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Letter-position coding in random consonant arrays , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[42]  N. F. Johnson,et al.  A Cohort Model of Visual Word Recognition , 1994, Cognitive Psychology.

[43]  N. Logothetis,et al.  View-dependent object recognition by monkeys , 1994, Current Biology.

[44]  C W McMonnies,et al.  Visuo-spatial discrimination and mirror image letter reversals in reading. , 1992, Journal of the American Optometric Association.

[45]  I. Biederman,et al.  Evidence for Complete Translational and Reflectional Invariance in Visual Object Priming , 1991, Perception.

[46]  Philip T Quinlan,et al.  Orthographic processing in visual word identification , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[47]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[48]  Y. Miyashita Neuronal correlate of visual associative long-term memory in the primate temporal cortex , 1988, Nature.

[49]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Attention and Performance XII: The Psychology of Reading , 1987 .

[50]  Mozer,et al.  Early parallel processing in reading: a connectionist approach. Technical report, April-November 1986 , 1986 .

[51]  James M. Cornell Spontaneous mirror-writing in children. , 1985 .

[52]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[53]  T Shallice,et al.  Word-form dyslexia. , 1980, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[54]  David R. Topper,et al.  Mirror-Image Confusion , 1979 .

[55]  H. Bouma Visual interference in the parafoveal recognition of initial and final letters of words. , 1973, Vision research.

[56]  G. M. Reicher Perceptual recognition as a function of meaninfulness of stimulus material. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[57]  D. Hubel,et al.  Cortical and callosal connections concerned with the vertical meridian of visual fields in the cat. , 1967, Journal of neurophysiology.