Modeling behavioral measures of error detection in choice tasks: response monitoring versus conflict monitoring.

The present study investigated the mechanisms underlying error detection in the error signaling response. The authors tested between a response monitoring account and a conflict monitoring account. By implementing each account within the neural network model of N. Yeung, M. M. Botvinick, and J. D. Cohen (2004), they demonstrated that both accounts make different predictions as to how error signaling performance is influenced by varying the participants' response criterion. These predictions were tested in an experiment using the Eriksen-flanker task. The qualitative pattern as well as a model fit favored the response monitoring account, which states that error detection is mediated by detecting internal error corrections.

[1]  P Rabbitt,et al.  Age, IQ and awareness, and recall of errors. , 1990, Ergonomics.

[2]  Patrick Rabbitt,et al.  Processing a Display Even after you Make a Response to it. How Perceptual Errors can be Corrected , 1981, Cognitive Development and the Ageing Process.

[3]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Dopamine and the mechanisms of cognition: Part II. D-amphetamine effects in human subjects performing a selective attention task , 1998, Biological Psychiatry.

[4]  P. Rabbitt Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[6]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions , 1998 .

[7]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.

[8]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  How does error correction differ from error signaling? An event-related potential study , 2006, Brain Research.

[9]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[10]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[11]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A parallel distributed processing approach to automaticity. , 1992, The American journal of psychology.

[12]  Marco Steinhauser,et al.  Response-based strengthening in task shifting: evidence from shift effects produced by errors. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  P. Rabbitt Time to detect errors as a function of factors affecting choice-response time. , 1967, Acta psychologica.

[14]  P. M. A. RABBITT,et al.  Error Correction Time without External Error Signals , 1966, Nature.

[15]  D. Laming Choice reaction performance following an error , 1979 .

[16]  T. Endrass,et al.  Error Awareness in a Saccade Countermanding Task , 2005 .

[17]  C. Brunia,et al.  Psychophysiological brain research. , 1993 .

[18]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[19]  Patrick Rabbitt,et al.  Consciousness is slower than you think , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[20]  D. Tucker,et al.  Medial Frontal Cortex in Action Monitoring , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  Cameron S. Carter,et al.  Dopamine and the mechanisms of cognition: Part I. A neural network model predicting dopamine effects on selective attention , 1998, Biological Psychiatry.

[22]  Rafal Bogacz,et al.  Parameterization of connectionist models , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[23]  J J Hopfield,et al.  Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. , 1982, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[24]  K M Spencer,et al.  The lateralized readiness potential: relationship between human data and response activation in a connectionist model. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[25]  E Berendsen,et al.  Goal neglect and inhibitory limitations: dissociable causes of interference effects in conflict situations. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[26]  Bryan Rodgers,et al.  What does a Man do after he Makes an Error? An Analysis of Response Programming , 1977 .

[27]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: evidence from an antisaccade task. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[28]  P M Rabbitt,et al.  Three Kinds of Error-Signalling Responses in a Serial Choice Task , 1968, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[29]  P Rabbitt,et al.  Some Errors of Perceptual Analysis in Visual Search can be Detected and Corrected , 1978, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[30]  M. Coles,et al.  "Where did I go wrong?" A psychophysiological analysis of error detection. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  P M Rabbitt,et al.  Error-Detection and Correction Latencies as a Function of S-R Compatibility , 1967, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[32]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Event-related potential correlates of errors in reaction tasks. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[33]  Richard Ridderinkhof Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: activation and suppression in conflict tasks , 2002, Psychological research.

[34]  H Pashler,et al.  Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  A mechanism for error detection in speeded response time tasks. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[36]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[37]  Katja Fiehler,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of error correction. , 2005, Psychophysiology.