“Mom called me!” Behavioral and prefrontal responses of infants to self-names spoken by their mothers

Development of a sense of self is a fundamental process needed for human social interaction. Although functional neuroimaging studies have revealed the importance of medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in self-referencing, how this function develops in infancy remains poorly understood. To determine the cerebral basis underlying processing of self-related stimuli, we used behavioral measures and functional multi-channel near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to measure prefrontal cortical responses in 6-month-old infants hearing their own names. We also investigated the influence of a mother's voice on name perception in infants - an ability that plays a crucial role in the recognition of social signals. Experiment 1 measured the behavioral preferences of infants for their own names and for other names, spoken either by their mothers or by strangers. Results showed that infants significantly preferred their own name to other names, regardless of speaker type. Experiment 2 examined hemodynamic responses to the same four conditions in the prefrontal cortex. Compared with other names, hearing their own names, especially when spoken by their mother, elicited greater activity in the infant's dorsal mPFC. Furthermore, the magnitude of the cerebral response correlated with the degree of behavioral preference only when involving their mother's voice. These findings suggest that, particularly in the context of their mothers' voice, the dorsal mPFC of infants is already sensitive to social signals related to self at 6 months. At the same time, familiarity and affection related processing are also discussed as possible factors modulating dorsal mPFC activation at this age.

[1]  A. Nakamura,et al.  Neural substrates for recognition of familiar voices: a PET study , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  Istvan Molnar-Szakacs,et al.  Self-face recognition activates a frontoparietal “mirror” network in the right hemisphere: an event-related fMRI study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[3]  D. Pisoni,et al.  Infants' Recognition of the Sound Patterns of Their Own Names , 1995, Psychological science.

[4]  A. de Rugy,et al.  Different mechanisms contributing to savings and anterograde interference are impaired in Parkinson's disease , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[5]  J. Karhu,et al.  Four-month-old infants process own mother's voice faster than unfamiliar voices--electrical signs of sensitization in infant brain. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[6]  Tobias Grossmann,et al.  The role of medial prefrontal cortex in early social cognition , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[7]  David M J S Bowman,et al.  Flammable biomes dominated by eucalypts originated at the Cretaceous-Palaeogene boundary. , 2011, Nature communications.

[8]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Neural mechanisms involved in the detection of our first name: a combined ERPs and PET study , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  Masako Okamoto,et al.  Three-dimensional probabilistic anatomical cranio-cerebral correlation via the international 10–20 system oriented for transcranial functional brain mapping , 2004, NeuroImage.

[10]  U. Frith Mind Blindness and the Brain in Autism , 2001, Neuron.

[11]  Pascal Belin,et al.  Mother and stranger: an electrophysiological study of voice processing in newborns. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  Claude Delpuech,et al.  Brain responses to a subject's own name uttered by a familiar voice , 2006, Brain Research.

[13]  Hideyuki Okano,et al.  Stem Cell-Like Properties of the Endometrial Side Population: Implication in Endometrial Regeneration , 2010, PloS one.

[15]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  The Detection of Communicative Signals Directed at the Self in Infant Prefrontal Cortex , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[16]  Agata Rozga,et al.  A prospective study of response to name in infants at risk for autism. , 2007, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[17]  Ippeita Dan,et al.  Referential framework for transcranial anatomical correspondence for fNIRS based on manually traced sulci and gyri of an infant brain , 2014, Neuroscience Research.

[18]  A. Decasper,et al.  Of Human Bonding: Newborns Prefer Their Mothers' Voices , 1980 .

[19]  Geraldine Dawson,et al.  Early recognition of 1-year-old infants with autism spectrum disorder versus mental retardation , 2002, Development and Psychopathology.

[20]  E. Watanabe,et al.  Language-specific cortical activation patterns for verbal fluency tasks in Japanese as assessed by multichannel functional near-infrared spectroscopy , 2013, Brain and Language.

[21]  A. Walker-Andrews,et al.  Mothers, fathers, and infants: the role of person familiarity and parental involvement in infants' perception of emotion expressions. , 2002, Child development.

[22]  Masako Okamoto,et al.  Virtual spatial registration of stand-alone fNIRS data to MNI space , 2007, NeuroImage.

[23]  Shozo Kojima,et al.  Cerebral responses to infant-directed speech and the effect of talker familiarity , 2012, NeuroImage.

[24]  Shigeru Sato,et al.  Cortical mechanism of communicative speech production , 2007, NeuroImage.

[25]  G. Taga,et al.  Brain imaging in awake infants by near-infrared optical topography , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  H. Jasper,et al.  The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. , 1999, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[27]  A. Endress,et al.  The Social Sense: Susceptibility to Others’ Beliefs in Human Infants and Adults , 2010, Science.

[28]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Neurology: Self-recognition and the right hemisphere , 2001, Nature.

[29]  C. Delpuech,et al.  Subject's own name as a novel in a MMN design: A combined ERP and PET study , 2008, Brain Research.

[30]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[31]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  The head-turn preference procedure for testing auditory perception , 1995 .

[32]  Myra A. Fernandes,et al.  Neural correlates of recollection and familiarity: A review of neuroimaging and patient data , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  F. Craik,et al.  In search of the emotional self: an fMRI study using positive and negative emotional words. , 2003, The American journal of psychiatry.

[34]  Reiko Mazuka,et al.  Brain responses in the processing of lexical pitch-accent by Japanese speakers , 2007, Neuroreport.

[35]  Masako Okamoto,et al.  Automated cortical projection of head-surface locations for transcranial functional brain mapping , 2005, NeuroImage.

[36]  C. Frith,et al.  “Hey John”: Signals Conveying Communicative Intention toward the Self Activate Brain Regions Associated with “Mentalizing,” Regardless of Modality , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Sing that tune: infants' perception of melody and lyrics and the facilitation of phonetic recognition in songs. , 2010, Infant behavior & development.

[38]  U. Neisser Five kinds of self‐knowledge , 1988 .

[39]  Yasuhiro Kanakogi,et al.  Developmental correspondence between action prediction and motor ability in early infancy. , 2011, Nature communications.

[40]  J. Mehler,et al.  Sounds and silence: An optical topography study of language recognition at birth , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[41]  A. Woodward Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.

[42]  Gergely Csibra,et al.  Recognizing Communicative Intentions in Infancy , 2010 .

[43]  Ippeita Dan,et al.  Prefrontal activation associated with social attachment: facial-emotion recognition in mothers and infants. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[44]  Rochelle S. Newman,et al.  Listen to your mother! The role of talker familiarity in infant streaming , 2004, Cognition.

[45]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Language or music, mother or Mozart? Structural and environmental influences on infants’ language networks , 2010, Brain and Language.

[46]  Georg Northoff,et al.  Self-referential processing in our brain—A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self , 2006, NeuroImage.

[47]  Effects of eye gaze cues provided by the caregiver compared to a stranger on infants’ object processing , 2012, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  E. Okada,et al.  Monte Carlo prediction of near-infrared light propagation in realistic adult and neonatal head models. , 2003, Applied optics.

[49]  F. Overwalle Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. , 2009 .

[50]  J. Morgan,et al.  Mommy and Me , 2005, Psychological science.

[51]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  “Did You Call Me?” 5-Month-Old Infants Own Name Guides Their Attention , 2010, PloS one.

[52]  M. Raichle,et al.  Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[53]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Measuring consciousness: relating behavioural and neurophysiological approaches , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[54]  D. Norman,et al.  Normal maturation of the neonatal and infant brain: MR imaging at 1.5 T. , 1988, Radiology.