Performance monitoring for sensorimotor confidence: A visuomotor tracking study

To best interact with the external world, humans are often required to consider the quality of their actions. Sometimes the environment furnishes rewards or punishments to signal action efficacy. However, when such feedback is absent or only partial, we must rely on internally generated signals to evaluate our performance (i.e., metacognition). Yet, very little is known about how humans form such judgements of sensorimotor confidence. Do they monitor their performance? Or do they rely on cues to sensorimotor uncertainty to infer how likely it is they performed well? We investigated motor metacognition in two visuomotor tracking experiments, where participants followed an unpredictably moving dot cloud with a mouse cursor as it followed a random trajectory. Their goal was to infer the underlying target generating the dots, track it for several seconds, and then report their confidence in their tracking as better or worse than their average. In Experiment 1, we manipulated task difficulty with two methods: varying the size of the dot cloud and varying the stability of the target’s velocity. In Experiment 2, the stimulus statistics were fixed and duration of the stimulus presentation was varied. We found similar levels of metacognitive sensitivity in all experiments, with the temporal analysis revealing a recency effect, where error later in the trial had a greater influence on the sensorimotor confidence. In sum, these results indicate humans predominantly monitor their tracking performance, albeit inefficiently, to judge sensorimotor confidence. Highlights Participants consciously reflected on their tracking performance with some accuracy Sensorimotor confidence was influenced by recent errors Expectations of task difficulty did not play a large role in sensorimotor confidence Metacognitive sensitivity of binary confidence judgements on continuous performance can be quantified with standard non-parametric techniques

[1]  M. Landy,et al.  Human online adaptation to changes in prior probability , 2018, bioRxiv.

[2]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Choice Certainty Is Informed by Both Evidence and Decision Time , 2014, Neuron.

[3]  Wei Ji Ma,et al.  Comparing Bayesian and non-Bayesian accounts of human confidence reports , 2018, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[4]  P. Mamassian Confidence Forced-Choice and Other Metaperceptual Tasks* , 2020, Perception.

[5]  Pascal Mamassian,et al.  Visual Confidence. , 2016, Annual review of vision science.

[6]  Simon Barthelmé,et al.  Flexible mechanisms underlie the evaluation of visual confidence , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[7]  E. Todorov Optimality principles in sensorimotor control , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  P. Vuilleumier,et al.  Inter-individual variability in metacognitive ability for visuomotor performance and underlying brain structures , 2015, Consciousness and Cognition.

[9]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[10]  Michael S Landy,et al.  Motor control is decision-making , 2012, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[11]  M. Usher,et al.  The demise of short-term memory revisited: empirical and computational investigations of recency effects. , 2005, Psychological review.

[12]  Donald A. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action , 1986 .

[13]  Dobromir Rahnev,et al.  How experimental procedures influence estimates of metacognitive ability , 2019, Neuroscience of consciousness.

[14]  Nathaniel D. Daw,et al.  Self-Evaluation of Decision-Making: A General Bayesian Framework for Metacognitive Computation , 2017, Psychological review.

[15]  Alexandre Pouget,et al.  Confidence and certainty: distinct probabilistic quantities for different goals , 2016, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  Callum D Mole,et al.  Metacognitive judgements of perceptual-motor steering performance , 2018, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  Pascal Mamassian,et al.  Weighting Mean and Variability during Confidence Judgments , 2015, PloS one.

[18]  M. Landy,et al.  It's that time again , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  H. Lau,et al.  How to measure metacognition , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[20]  Konrad P. Körding,et al.  Bayesian Integration and Non-Linear Feedback Control in a Full-Body Motor Task , 2009, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[21]  Johannes Burge,et al.  Continuous psychophysics: Target-tracking to measure visual sensitivity. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[22]  Hakwan Lau,et al.  The signal processing architecture underlying subjective reports of sensory awareness , 2016, Neuroscience of consciousness.

[23]  D. Patel,et al.  Inferring subjective states through the observation of actions , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[24]  J. Townsend,et al.  Decision field theory: a dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment. , 1993, Psychological review.

[25]  Ariel Zylberberg,et al.  The construction of confidence in a perceptual decision , 2012, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[26]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Changing your mind: a computational mechanism of vacillation , 2009, Nature.

[27]  David L. Kleinman,et al.  Optimal control of linear systems with time-delay and observation noise , 1969 .

[28]  R. Dolan,et al.  The neural basis of metacognitive ability , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[29]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[30]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[31]  Richard C. Atkinson,et al.  Human Memory: A Proposed System and its Control Processes , 1968, Psychology of Learning and Motivation.

[32]  Aude Oliva,et al.  Estimating perception of scene layout properties from global image features. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[33]  G. Schwartz,et al.  Consciousness and Self-Regulation , 1976 .

[34]  R. Miall,et al.  Intermittency in human manual tracking tasks. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[35]  J V Baranski,et al.  Probing the locus of confidence judgments: experiments on the time to determine confidence. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Saccadic localization of random dot targets , 1998, Vision Research.

[37]  Susan J. Galvin,et al.  Type 2 tasks in the theory of signal detectability: Discrimination between correct and incorrect decisions , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[38]  Hang Zhang,et al.  Testing Whether Humans Have an Accurate Model of Their Own Motor Uncertainty in a Speeded Reaching Task , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[39]  Christopher Summerfield,et al.  Metacognition in human decision-making: confidence and error monitoring , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[40]  Emiliano Santarnecchi,et al.  TMS Interference with Primacy and Recency Mechanisms Reveals Bimodal Episodic Encoding in the Human Brain , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Abnormalities in the awareness of action , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  Yasuharu Koike,et al.  A Biased Bayesian Inference for Decision-Making and Cognitive Control , 2018, Front. Neurosci..

[43]  Derek H. Arnold,et al.  Computations underlying confidence in visual perception. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[45]  H. Lau,et al.  A signal detection theoretic approach for estimating metacognitive sensitivity from confidence ratings , 2012, Consciousness and Cognition.

[46]  Robert C. Wilson,et al.  An Approximately Bayesian Delta-Rule Model Explains the Dynamics of Belief Updating in a Changing Environment , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  Manish Singh,et al.  Robust visual estimation as source separation. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[48]  Wei Ji Ma,et al.  Comparing Bayesian and non-Bayesian accounts of human confidence reports , 2018 .

[49]  P. Haggard,et al.  Evidence for metacognitive bias in perception of voluntary action , 2018, bioRxiv.

[50]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[51]  The effect of judgment type and confidence scale on confidence-accuracy calibration in face recognition. , 2003, The Journal of applied psychology.

[52]  Timothy J. Pleskac,et al.  Two-stage dynamic signal detection: a theory of choice, decision time, and confidence. , 2010, Psychological review.

[53]  William H. Alexander,et al.  Computational Models of Performance Monitoring and Cognitive Control , 2010, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[54]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[55]  David A. Rosenbaum,et al.  Metacognitive control of action: Preparation for aiming reflects knowledge of Fitts’s law , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[56]  C. Frith,et al.  Self-awareness and action , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[57]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  Behavioral, Modeling, and Electrophysiological Evidence for Supramodality in Human Metacognition , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[59]  R. Passingham Attention to action. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[60]  Zoltan Dienes,et al.  Measures of metacognition on signal-detection theoretic models. , 2013, Psychological methods.

[61]  M. Landy,et al.  Decision making, movement planning and statistical decision theory , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[62]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.

[63]  P. Mamassian Overconfidence in an Objective Anticipatory Motor Task , 2008, Psychological science.

[64]  Bingni W. Brunton,et al.  Rats and Humans Can Optimally Accumulate Evidence for Decision-Making , 2013, Science.

[65]  Simon Barthelmé,et al.  Spatial statistics and attentional dynamics in scene viewing. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[66]  Rebekah L. Blakemore,et al.  Metacognition of visuomotor decisions in conversion disorder , 2018, Neuropsychologia.

[67]  Florent Meyniel,et al.  On-line confidence monitoring during decision making , 2018, Cognition.

[68]  A. Moran,et al.  Metacognition and action: a new pathway to understanding social and cognitive aspects of expertise in sport , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[69]  R. Gregory The Most Expensive Painting in the World , 2007, Perception.