Color appearance under conditions of chromatic adaptation and contrast

Chromatic adaptation can significantly alter the color appearance of a light. We review measurements of changes in appearance caused by chromatic backgrounds and surrounds. Effects of long-, middle- and short-wavelength adapting lights are assessed by having observers adjust the physical light in a test patch to maintain a constant perceptual criterion: a test that appears neither reddish nor greenish. A variety of spatial and temporal stimulus configurations also is considered. An integrated account of the measurements is provided by the two-process theory of chromatic adaptation, originally proposed by Hurvich and Jameson in 1958, which specifies an adapting light alters receptoral sensitivities and, simultaneously, contributes an additive increment to opponent chromatic responses.

[1]  S. Shevell Color perception under chromatic adaptation: Equilibrium yellow and long-wavelength adaptation , 1982, Vision Research.

[2]  B. Drum Additive effect of backgrounds in chromatic induction , 1981, Vision Research.

[3]  James Larimer,et al.  Red/green opponent colors equilibria measured on chromatic adapting fields: Evidence for gain changes and restoring forces , 1981, Vision Research.

[4]  J. Pokorny,et al.  Spectral sensitivity of the foveal cone photopigments between 400 and 500 nm , 1975, Vision Research.

[5]  J. J. Vos,et al.  Light profiles of the foveal image of a point source , 1976, Vision Research.

[6]  J. Walraven Discounting the background—the missing link in the explanation of chromatic induction , 1976, Vision Research.

[7]  A. Gilchrist,et al.  The classification and integration of edges as critical to the perception of reflectance and illumination , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[8]  W. Stiles The Directional Sensitivity of the Retina and the Spectral Sensitivities of the Rods and Cones , 1939 .

[9]  C. M. Cicerone,et al.  Opponent-process additivity--I: red-green equilibria. , 1974, Vision research.

[10]  E. Adelson Looking at the world through a rose-colored ganzfeld , 1981, Vision Research.

[11]  F. Campbell,et al.  Optical quality of the human eye , 1966, The Journal of physiology.

[12]  L. Arend,et al.  Simultaneous color constancy. , 1986, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[13]  S. Shevell,et al.  Color perception with binocularly fused adapting fields of different wavelengths , 1985, Vision Research.

[14]  W. Cowan,et al.  Changes in perceived color due to chromatic interactions , 1982, Vision Research.

[15]  S. Shevell The dual role of chromatic backgrounds in color perception , 1978, Vision Research.

[16]  John Krauskopf,et al.  Light Distribution in Human Retinal Images , 1962 .

[17]  S. Shevell,et al.  Color perception under chromatic adaptation: Red/green equilibria with adapted short-wavelength-sensitive cones , 1988, Vision Research.

[18]  Steven K. Shevell,et al.  Light spread and scatter from some common adapting stimuli: Computations based on the point-source light profile , 1988, Vision Research.

[19]  Steven K. Shevell,et al.  Unambiguous evidence for the additive effect in chromatic adaptation , 1980, Vision Research.

[20]  J. Walraven,et al.  No additive effect of backgrounds in chromatic induction , 1979, Vision Research.

[21]  S. McKee,et al.  Quantitative studies in retinex theory a comparison between theoretical predictions and observer responses to the “color mondrian” experiments , 1976, Vision Research.

[22]  H. Walters Some experiments on the trichromatic theory of vision , 1942, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B - Biological Sciences.