The co-evolution of culturally inherited altruistic helping and cultural transmission under random group formation.

Limited migration results in kin selective pressure on helping behaviors under a wide range of ecological, demographic and life-history situations. However, such genetically determined altruistic helping can evolve only when migration is not too strong and group size is not too large. Cultural inheritance of helping behaviors may allow altruistic helping to evolve in groups of larger size because cultural transmission has the potential to markedly decrease the variance within groups and augment the variance between groups. Here, we study the co-evolution of culturally inherited altruistic helping behaviors and two alternative cultural transmission rules for such behaviors. We find that conformist transmission, where individuals within groups tend to copy prevalent cultural variants (e.g., beliefs or values), has a strong adverse effect on the evolution of culturally inherited helping traits. This finding is at variance with the commonly held view that conformist transmission is a crucial factor favoring the evolution of altruistic helping in humans. By contrast, we find that under one-to-many transmission, where individuals within groups tend to copy a "leader" (or teacher), altruistic helping can evolve in groups of any size, although the cultural transmission rule itself hitchhikes rather weakly with a selected helping trait. Our results suggest that culturally determined helping behaviors are more likely to be driven by "leaders" than by popularity, but the emergence and stability of the cultural transmission rules themselves should be driven by some extrinsic factors.

[1]  L. Cavalli-Sforza Cultural transmission and evolution , 1981 .

[2]  L. Keller,et al.  Strong Reciprocity or Strong Ferocity? A Population Genetic View of the Evolution of Altruistic Punishment , 2007, The American Naturalist.

[3]  Brian Charlesworth,et al.  A Note on the Evolution of Altruism in Structured Demes , 1979, The American Naturalist.

[4]  Joe Yuichiro Wakano,et al.  A mixed strategy model for the emergence and intensification of social learning in a periodically changing natural environment. , 2006, Theoretical population biology.

[5]  U. Dieckmann,et al.  THE ADAPTIVE DYNAMICS OF ALTRUISM IN SPATIALLY HETEROGENEOUS POPULATIONS , 2003, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[6]  M. Toro,et al.  Cultural Transmission and Evolution , 2010 .

[7]  Elhanan Borenstein,et al.  EVOLUTION OF LEARNING IN FLUCTUATING ENVIRONMENTS: WHEN SELECTION FAVORS BOTH SOCIAL AND EXPLORATORY INDIVIDUAL LEARNING , 2008, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[8]  I. Eshel,et al.  On the founder effect and the evolution of altruistic traits. , 1976, Theoretical population biology.

[9]  M W Feldman,et al.  Models for cultural inheritance. I. Group mean and within group variation. , 1973, Theoretical population biology.

[10]  Joe Yuichiro Wakano,et al.  Evolution of social learning: a mathematical analysis. , 2004, Theoretical population biology.

[11]  A Grafen,et al.  An inclusive fitness analysis of altruism on a cyclical network , 2007, Journal of evolutionary biology.

[12]  Peter J. Richerson,et al.  Cultural transmission and the evolution of cooperative behavior , 1982 .

[13]  V. Jansen,et al.  Altruism through beard chromodynamics , 2006, Nature.

[14]  U. Fischbacher,et al.  Strong reciprocity, human cooperation, and the enforcement of social norms , 2002, Human nature.

[15]  Kenichi Aoki,et al.  A CONDITION FOR GROUP SELECTION TO PREVAIL OVER COUNTERACTING INDIVIDUAL SELECTION , 1982, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[16]  George R. Price,et al.  Selection and Covariance , 1970, Nature.

[17]  John Tyler Bonner The evolution of culture in animals , 1958 .

[18]  I. Eshel On the neighbor effect and the evolution of altruistic traits. , 1972, Theoretical population biology.

[19]  Ross A Hammond,et al.  ALTRUISM VIA KIN‐SELECTION STRATEGIES THAT RELY ON ARBITRARY TAGS WITH WHICH THEY COEVOLVE , 2004, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[20]  P. Richerson,et al.  The Origin and Evolution of Cultures , 2005 .

[21]  Ken Binmore,et al.  A Little Behavioralism Can Go A Long Way , 2006 .

[22]  M W Feldman,et al.  Theories of kin and group selection: a population genetics perspective. , 1980, Theoretical population biology.

[23]  Ken Binmore,et al.  Does Game Theory Work? The Bargaining Challenge , 2007 .

[24]  W. Hamilton The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I. , 1964, Journal of theoretical biology.

[25]  Laurent Lehmann,et al.  Cultural Transmission Can Inhibit the Evolution of Altruistic Helping , 2008, The American Naturalist.

[26]  P. Allison Cultural relatedness under oblique and horizontal transmission rules , 1992 .

[27]  J. Henrich Cultural group selection, coevolutionary processes and large-scale cooperation , 2004 .

[28]  A. Grafen Natural selection, kin selection and group selection [Polistes fuscatus, wasps] , 1984 .

[29]  J. Goodall,et al.  Tool-Using and Aimed Throwing in a Community of Free-Living Chimpanzees , 1964, Nature.

[30]  Wataru Nakahashi,et al.  The evolution of conformist transmission in social learning when the environment changes periodically. , 2007, Theoretical population biology.

[31]  Alan Grafen,et al.  Detecting kin selection at work using inclusive fitness , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[32]  E. Rogers Diffusion of Innovations , 1962 .

[33]  Jack P. Hailman,et al.  Man and Beast: Comparative Social Behavior , 1971 .

[34]  E. Wilson,et al.  Translation of epigenetic rules of individual behavior into ethnographic patterns. , 1980, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[35]  A. Gardner,et al.  Demography, altruism, and the benefits of budding , 2006, Journal of evolutionary biology.

[36]  W. Hamilton Innate social aptitudes of man: an approach from evolutionary genetics , 1975 .

[37]  R. Buerger The Mathematical Theory of Selection, Recombination, and Mutation , 2000 .

[38]  L. Lehmann,et al.  The evolution of trans‐generational altruism: kin selection meets niche construction , 2007, Journal of evolutionary biology.

[39]  P. Richerson,et al.  Not by Genes Alone , 2004 .

[40]  P. Richerson,et al.  Culture and the Evolutionary Process , 1988 .

[41]  Edward O. Wilson,et al.  GENES, MIND AND CULTURE , 1984 .

[42]  W. Ewens Mathematical Population Genetics , 1980 .

[43]  F. Rousset,et al.  CONSTRAINTS ON THE ORIGIN AND MAINTENANCE OF GENETIC KIN RECOGNITION , 2007, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[44]  Alan R. Rogers,et al.  Group Selection by Selective Emigration: The Effects of Migration and Kin Structure , 1990, The American Naturalist.

[45]  P D Taylor,et al.  OVERLAPPING GENERATIONS CAN PROMOTE ALTRUISTIC BEHAVIOR , 2000, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[46]  R Boyd,et al.  Why people punish defectors. Weak conformist transmission can stabilize costly enforcement of norms in cooperative dilemmas. , 2001, Journal of theoretical biology.

[47]  W. Hamilton,et al.  Selfish and Spiteful Behaviour in an Evolutionary Model , 1970, Nature.

[48]  H. Ohtsuki,et al.  A simple rule for the evolution of cooperation on graphs and social networks , 2006, Nature.

[49]  P. Taylor Altruism in viscous populations — an inclusive fitness model , 1992, Evolutionary Ecology.

[50]  M. Baalen,et al.  The Unit of Selection in Viscous Populations and the Evolution of Altruism. , 1998, Journal of theoretical biology.

[51]  F. Rousset Genetic Structure and Selection in Subdivided Populations (MPB-40) , 2004 .

[52]  S. Frank Foundations of Social Evolution , 2019 .

[53]  R. Bshary,et al.  Distinguishing four fundamental approaches to the evolution of helping , 2008, Journal of evolutionary biology.

[54]  Ken Binmore,et al.  Why do people cooperate? , 2006 .

[55]  M. Baalen,et al.  Self-structuring in spatial evolutionary ecology. , 2008, Ecology letters.