Feeling more than we can know: Exposure effects without learning.

[1]  D. Stang Effects of "mere exposure" on learning and affect. , 1975, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  Robert B. Zajonc,et al.  Exposure effects and associative learning , 1974 .

[3]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. , 1977 .

[4]  R. S. Corteen,et al.  Autonomic responses to shock-associated words in an unattended channel. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  W. C. Howell Representation of frequency in memory. , 1973 .

[6]  Stephen Kaplan,et al.  A VERSATILE DEVICE FOR THE MEASUREMENT OF SKIN-RESISTANCE IN RATS AND HUMANS. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.

[7]  J. Crandall Familiarity, preference, and expectancy arousal. , 1967, Journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  The Processing of Frequency Information: An Automatic Mechanism?. , 1977 .

[9]  Howard Shevrin,et al.  Average evoked response and verbal correlates of unconscious mental processes. , 1971, Psychophysiology.

[10]  B. Mellers,et al.  Stimulus Recognition May Mediate Exposure Effects , 1979 .

[11]  L. Festinger Social pressures in informal groups : a study of human factors in housing / by Leon Festinger, Stanley Schachter and Kurt Back , 1950 .

[12]  P. Vitz Affect as a function of stimulus variation. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[13]  Mady Wechsler Segal,et al.  Alphabet and attraction: An unobtrusive measure of the effect of propinquity in a field setting. , 1974 .

[14]  R. Zajonc Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. , 1968 .

[15]  A. Harrison,et al.  Response competition, frequency, exploratory behavior, and liking. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  N. Moray Attention in Dichotic Listening: Affective Cues and the Influence of Instructions , 1959 .

[17]  M. Erdelyi A new look at the new look: perceptual defense and vigilance. , 1974, Psychological review.

[18]  D. Stang Intuition as artifact in mere exposure studies. , 1974 .

[19]  J. Lewis,et al.  NOVELTY, COMPLEXITY, INCONGRUITY, EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION, AND THE GSR. , 1963, Journal of experimental psychology.

[20]  S. M. Sales,et al.  Attitudinal effects of “mere exposure”: A reevaluation , 1971 .

[21]  H. Shevrin,et al.  Visual Evoked Response Correlates of Unconscious Mental Processes , 1968, Science.

[22]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Discrimination and learning without awareness: a methodological survey and evaluation. , 1960, Psychological review.

[23]  T. Bever,et al.  Cerebral Dominance in Musicians and Nonmusicians , 1974, Science.

[24]  V. Konečni,et al.  Awareness in verbal nonoperant conditioning: an approach through dichotic listening. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.

[25]  R. Zajonc,et al.  Exposure, context, and interpersonal attraction. , 1973 .

[26]  E. Martin,et al.  Stimulus meaningfulness and paired-associate transfer: an encoding variability hypothesis. , 1968, Psychological review.

[27]  D. Berlyne Novelty, complexity, and hedonic value , 1970 .

[28]  D. Lawrence The nature of a stimulus: Some relationships between learning and perception. , 1962 .

[29]  A. W. Melton,et al.  Coding Processes in Human Memory. , 1973 .

[30]  M W Matlin,et al.  Response competition, recognition, and affect. , 1971, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[31]  D. Kimura,et al.  The asymmetry of the human brain. , 1973, Scientific American.

[32]  P. Vitz,et al.  PREFERENCES FOR RATES OF INFORMATION PRESENTED BY SEQUENCES OF TONES. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.

[33]  William C. Howell,et al.  Storage of events and event frequencies: A comparison of two paradigms in memory. , 1973 .

[34]  R. Moreland,et al.  Is stimulus recognition a necessary condition for the occurrence of exposure effects , 1977 .