Unlearning versus savings in visuomotor adaptation: comparing effects of washout, passage of time, and removal of errors on motor memory

Humans are able to rapidly adapt their movements when a visuomotor or other systematic perturbation is imposed. However, the adaptation is forgotten or unlearned equally rapidly once the perturbation is removed. The ultimate cause of this unlearning remains poorly understood. Unlearning is often considered to be a passive process due to inability to retain an internal model. However, we have recently suggested that it may instead be a process of reversion to habit, without necessarily any forgetting per se. We compared the timecourse and nature of unlearning across a variety of protocols where unlearning is known to occur: error-clamp trials, removal of visual feedback, removal of the perturbation, or simply a period of inactivity. We found that, in agreement with mathematical models, there was no significant difference in the rate of decay between subject who experienced zero-error clamp trials, and subjects who made movements with no visual feedback. Time alone did lead to partial unlearning (over the duration we tested), but the amount of unlearning was inconsistent across subjects. Upon re-exposure to the same perturbation, subjects who unlearned through time or by reverting to veridical feedback exhibited savings. By contrast, no savings was observed in subjects who unlearned by having visual feedback removed or by being placed in a series of error-clamp trials. Thus although these various forms of unlearning can all revert subjects back to baseline behavior, they have markedly different effects on whether long-term memory for the adaptation is spared or is also unlearned. On the basis of these and previous findings, we suggest that unlearning is not due to passive forgetting of an internal model, but is instead an active process whereby adapted behavior gradually reverts to baseline habits.

[1]  A. Haith,et al.  Model-based and model-free mechanisms of human motor learning. , 2013, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.

[2]  Raymond J. Delnicki,et al.  Overcoming Motor “Forgetting” Through Reinforcement Of Learned Actions , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Konrad P. Körding,et al.  Estimating the Relevance of World Disturbances to Explain Savings, Interference and Long-Term Motor Adaptation Effects , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[4]  Sarah E. Pekny,et al.  Protection and Expression of Human Motor Memories , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  P. Celnik,et al.  Dissociating the roles of the cerebellum and motor cortex during adaptive learning: the motor cortex retains what the cerebellum learns. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  Vincent S. Huang,et al.  Rethinking Motor Learning and Savings in Adaptation Paradigms: Model-Free Memory for Successful Actions Combines with Internal Models , 2011, Neuron.

[7]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learning from Sensory and Reward Prediction Errors during Motor Adaptation , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[8]  R. Ivry,et al.  An Explicit Strategy Prevails When the Cerebellum Fails to Compute Movement Errors , 2010, The Cerebellum.

[9]  Konrad P. Körding,et al.  Uncertainty of Feedback and State Estimation Determines the Speed of Motor Adaptation , 2009, Front. Comput. Neurosci..

[10]  N. Schweighofer,et al.  Dual Adaptation Supports a Parallel Architecture of Motor Memory , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Explaining savings for visuomotor adaptation: linear time-invariant state-space models are not sufficient. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Sarah E. Criscimagna-Hemminger,et al.  Consolidation Patterns of Human Motor Memory , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  J. Krakauer,et al.  A computational neuroanatomy for motor control , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  The dynamics of memory as a consequence of optimal adaptation to a changing body , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  A. Bastian Learning to predict the future: the cerebellum adapts feedforward movement control , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[17]  Henrik Jörntell,et al.  Synaptic Memories Upside Down: Bidirectional Plasticity at Cerebellar Parallel Fiber-Purkinje Cell Synapses , 2006, Neuron.

[18]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.

[19]  J. Krakauer,et al.  An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Modeling Sensorimotor Learning with Linear Dynamical Systems , 2006, Neural Computation.

[21]  Hannah J. Block,et al.  Interlimb coordination during locomotion: what can be adapted and stored? , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Adaptation to Visuomotor Transformations: Consolidation, Interference, and Forgetting , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  C. Hansel,et al.  Bidirectional Parallel Fiber Plasticity in the Cerebellum under Climbing Fiber Control , 2004, Neuron.

[24]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Quantifying Generalization from Trial-by-Trial Behavior of Adaptive Systems that Learn with Basis Functions: Theory and Experiments in Human Motor Control , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives , 2000, Nature.

[26]  R A Scheidt,et al.  Persistence of motor adaptation during constrained, multi-joint, arm movements. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  Mirsad Hadzikadic,et al.  Learning to Predict: INC2.5 , 1997, IEEE Trans. Knowl. Data Eng..

[28]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Parameter estimation for linear dynamical systems , 1996 .

[29]  P.J. Denning,et al.  On learning how to predict , 1980, Proceedings of the IEEE.