Revisiting the effects of Parkinson's disease and frontal lobe lesions on task switching: the role of rule reconfiguration.

This study investigated the hypothesis that rule reconfiguration in task switching can isolate aspects of intact and impaired control at different stages of Parkinson's disease (PD) by comparing switches between concrete naming rules pertaining to stimulus selection, to switches between abstract rules which allocate categorization responses to these stimuli. Based on previous findings, it was hypothesized that attentional switches, where task set competition emerges at the stimulus but not response set level, highlights striatal dopaminergic function. Conversely, increasing the degree of task set competition to encompass reconfiguration of response set when switching between abstract rules, represents a condition which engages the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and renders this manipulation sensitive to frontal damage. To this end, we investigated task switching with concrete and abstract rules in unilaterally (Hoehn & Yahr stage I) and bilaterally (Hoehn & Yahr stage II) affected PD patients, as well as striatally intact frontal lesion patients. As predicted, frontal lesion patients demonstrated switching deficits only with abstract categorization rules, where switching engendered complete task set reconfiguration and a new response, as did stage II PD patients with presumed frontal cortical pathology. Replicating previous findings, stage I PD patients with relatively circumscribed striatal pathology demonstrated no such impairment. Disease severity also impacted on attentional switching indexed by naming rules, since medicated stage II but not stage I patients demonstrated switching deficits emerging from stimulus set reconfiguration, suggesting that the ameliorative efficacy of dopaminergic medication is inversely related to the severity of the striatal deficit. These findings illustrate that the nature of the rules that are switched, and its implication in terms of reconfiguring different task set elements, highlights different neural characters of cognitive flexibility. These manipulations may help decipher the differential effects of progressive neurodegeneration on parkinsonian cognition, and provide a framework in which to conceptualize the contributions of cortical and subcortical regions to cognitive control.

[1]  Angie A. Kehagia,et al.  Neuropsychological and clinical heterogeneity of cognitive impairment and dementia in patients with Parkinson's disease , 2010, The Lancet Neurology.

[2]  Angie A. Kehagia,et al.  Learning and cognitive flexibility: frontostriatal function and monoaminergic modulation , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[3]  Nick Yeung,et al.  Bottom-up influences on voluntary task switching: the elusive homunculus escapes. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[4]  Roshan Cools,et al.  Switching between abstract rules reflects disease severity but not dopaminergic status in Parkinson's disease , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  E. Koechlin,et al.  Anterior Prefrontal Function and the Limits of Human Decision-Making , 2007, Science.

[6]  C. Summerfield,et al.  An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  D. Tranel,et al.  Impaired behavior on real-world tasks following damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex , 2007, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[8]  Barbara J. Knowlton,et al.  Backward inhibition in Parkinson's disease , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  Günther Deuschl,et al.  The impact of normal aging and Parkinson's disease on response preparation in task-switching behavior , 2006, Brain Research.

[10]  G. Mangun,et al.  Brain regions activated by endogenous preparatory set shifting as revealed by fMRI , 2006, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  Jonathan D. Wallis,et al.  A Comparison of Abstract Rules in the Prefrontal Cortex, Premotor Cortex, Inferior Temporal Cortex, and Striatum , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  C. Liston,et al.  Anterior Cingulate and Posterior Parietal Cortices Are Sensitive to Dissociable Forms of Conflict in a Task-Switching Paradigm , 2006, Neuron.

[13]  David J. Brooks,et al.  Imaging in Parkinson’s Disease: The Role of Monoamines in Behavior , 2006, Biological Psychiatry.

[14]  David Badre,et al.  Computational and neurobiological mechanisms underlying cognitive flexibility. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[15]  A. Miyake,et al.  Not All Executive Functions Are Related to Intelligence , 2006, Psychological science.

[16]  L. Nystrom,et al.  Between-Task Competition and Cognitive Control in Task Switching , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  M. Brass,et al.  Advance preparation and stimulus-induced interference in cued task switching: further insights from BOLD fMRI , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  Thomas R. Knösche,et al.  Who Comes First? The Role of the Prefrontal and Parietal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  Eveline A. Crone,et al.  Retrieving rules for behavior from long-term memory , 2005, NeuroImage.

[20]  Radek Ptak,et al.  The Inferior Temporal Lobe Mediates Distracter-Resistant Visual Search of Patients with Spatial Neglect , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  P. Pollux,et al.  Advance preparation of set-switches in Parkinson’s disease , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  S. Bunge How we use rules to select actions: A review of evidence from cognitive neuroscience , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[23]  T. Robbins,et al.  A componential analysis of task-switching deficits associated with lesions of left and right frontal cortex. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  M. Brass,et al.  Decomposing Components of Task Preparation with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Don't think of a white bear: An fMRI investigation of the effects of sequential instructional sets on cortical activity in a task‐switching paradigm , 2004, Human brain mapping.

[26]  T. Robbins,et al.  l-Dopa medication remediates cognitive inflexibility, but increases impulsivity in patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  Marcel Brass,et al.  When the same response has different meanings: recoding the response meaning in the lateral prefrontal cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.

[28]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Transient and Sustained Cognitive Control during Task Switching , 2003, Neuron.

[29]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Task-Set Switching Deficits in Early-Stage Huntington's Disease: Implications for Basal Ganglia Function , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  G. Logan,et al.  Clever homunculus: is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  H. Braak,et al.  Staging of brain pathology related to sporadic Parkinson’s disease , 2003, Neurobiology of Aging.

[32]  D. Bub,et al.  Task switching deficits associated with Parkinson’s disease reflect depleted attentional resources , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  Matthew F. S. Rushworth,et al.  Components of Switching Intentional Set , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Eliot Hazeltine,et al.  Dissociable Contributions of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices to Response Selection , 2002, NeuroImage.

[35]  K. A. Hadland,et al.  Role of the human medial frontal cortex in task switching: a combined fMRI and TMS study. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  T. Robbins,et al.  Mechanisms of cognitive set flexibility in Parkinson's disease. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[37]  T. Robbins,et al.  Enhanced or impaired cognitive function in Parkinson's disease as a function of dopaminergic medication and task demands. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[38]  A. Kramer,et al.  Changes in executive control across the life span: examination of task-switching performance. , 2001, Developmental psychology.

[39]  L. Bäckman,et al.  Dopamine and cognitive functioning: brain imaging findings in Huntington's disease and normal aging. , 2001, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[40]  U. Mayr Age differences in the selection of mental sets: the role of inhibition, stimulus ambiguity, and response-set overlap. , 2001, Psychology and aging.

[41]  D. Neary,et al.  Longitudinal evaluation of cognitive disorder in Huntington's disease , 2001, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[42]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[43]  R. D Rogers,et al.  Impaired planning but intact decision making in early Huntington’s disease: implications for specific fronto-striatal pathology , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  P. Strick,et al.  Basal ganglia and cerebellar loops: motor and cognitive circuits , 2000, Brain Research Reviews.

[45]  U. Lindenberger,et al.  Adult age differences in task switching. , 2000, Psychology and aging.

[46]  D Y von Cramon,et al.  Executive control functions in task switching: evidence from brain injured patients. , 1999, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[47]  A F Kramer,et al.  Task coordination and aging: explorations of executive control processes in the task switching paradigm. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[48]  Peter Redgrave,et al.  The basal ganglia: a vertebrate solution to the selection problem? , 1999, Neuroscience.

[49]  P. Rabbitt,et al.  A study of performance on tests from the CANTAB battery sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in a large sample of normal volunteers: Implications for theories of executive functioning and cognitive aging , 1998, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[50]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociating executive mechanisms of task control following frontal lobe damage and Parkinson's disease. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[51]  R Kliegl,et al.  Sequential and coordinative complexity in time-accuracy functions for mental arithmetic. , 1997, Psychology and aging.

[52]  L. Bäckman,et al.  Cognitive deficits in Huntington's disease are predicted by dopaminergic PET markers and brain volumes. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[53]  D. Head,et al.  Selective aging of the human cerebral cortex observed in vivo: differential vulnerability of the prefrontal gray matter. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[54]  J. Mink THE BASAL GANGLIA: FOCUSED SELECTION AND INHIBITION OF COMPETING MOTOR PROGRAMS , 1996, Progress in Neurobiology.

[55]  T. Robbins,et al.  Executive and mnemonic functions in early Huntington's disease. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[56]  R. West,et al.  An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[57]  C. Pantelis,et al.  Cognitive flexibility and complex integration in Parkinson's disease, Huntington's disease, and Schizophrenia , 1995, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[58]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[59]  C. Umilta,et al.  Attention and Control Deficits Following Closed Head Injury , 1994, Cortex.

[60]  R. Reitan,et al.  A selective and critical review of neuropsychological deficits and the frontal lobes , 1994, Neuropsychology Review.

[61]  Arthur P. Shimamura,et al.  Memory interference effects and aging: Findings from a test of frontal lobe function. , 1994 .

[62]  P N Leigh,et al.  Cognitive deficits in progressive supranuclear palsy, Parkinson's disease, and multiple system atrophy in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction. , 1994, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[63]  T. Robbins,et al.  Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinson's disease. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[64]  H J Sagar,et al.  Alternating fluency in Parkinson's disease. An evaluation of the attentional control theory of cognitive impairment. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[65]  M Richards,et al.  Executive function in Parkinson's disease: set-shifting or set-maintenance? , 1993, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[66]  S. Channon,et al.  Cognitive strategies and hypothesis testing during discrimination learning in Parkinson's disease , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[67]  C. Marsden,et al.  Fronto-striatal cognitive deficits at different stages of Parkinson's disease. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[68]  B. Horwitz,et al.  Interregional correlations of resting cerebral glucose metabolism in old and young women , 1992, Brain Research.

[69]  H. Whitaker,et al.  An empirical test of two opposing theoretical models of prefrontal function , 1992, Brain and Cognition.

[70]  C. Coffey,et al.  Quantitative cerebral anatomy of the aging human brain , 1992, Neurology.

[71]  F. N. Dempster,et al.  The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging , 1992 .

[72]  H. Damasio,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance as a measure of frontal lobe damage. , 1991, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[73]  T. Shallice,et al.  Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[74]  C. Marsden,et al.  Prism adaptation and other tasks involving spatial abilities in patients with Parkinson's disease, patients with frontal lobe lesions and patients with unilateral temporal lobectomies , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[75]  M M Mesulam,et al.  Large‐scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory , 1990, Annals of neurology.

[76]  K. Flowers,et al.  Motor set in Parkinson's disease. , 1990, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[77]  A. C. Roberts,et al.  Impaired extra-dimensional shift performance in medicated and unmedicated Parkinson's disease: Evidence for a specific attentional dysfunction , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[78]  T. Shallice From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure: Converging Operations: Specific Syndromes and Evidence from Normal Subjects , 1988 .

[79]  T. Robbins,et al.  Planning and spatial working memory in Parkinson's disease. , 1988, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[80]  T. Robbins,et al.  A comparative study of visuospatial memory and learning in Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinson's disease. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[81]  R. Barker,et al.  How does the brain control its own activity? A new function for the basal ganglia. , 1988, Journal of theoretical biology.

[82]  C. Marsden,et al.  Internal versus external cues and the control of attention in Parkinson's disease. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[83]  C. Marsden,et al.  'Frontal' cognitive function in patients with Parkinson's disease 'on' and 'off' levodopa. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[84]  D. C. Howell Statistical Methods for Psychology , 1987 .

[85]  J. Saint-Cyr,et al.  Frontal lobe dysfunction in Parkinson's disease. The cortical focus of neostriatal outflow. , 1986, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[86]  J. Duncan Disorganisation of behaviour after frontal lobe damage , 1986 .

[87]  A. Lang,et al.  Striatal dopamine distribution in Parkinsonian patients during life , 1985, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[88]  K. Flowers,et al.  The effect of Parkinson's disease on the ability to maintain a mental set. , 1985, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[89]  A. Cools Basal ganglia and parkinson's disease: neurobiological and pharmacological aspects in animals and man , 1984, Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery.

[90]  I. Biederman,et al.  Mental set and mental shift revisited , 1976 .

[91]  S. Folstein,et al.  “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician , 1975 .

[92]  M. M. Burns,et al.  Parkinsonism , 1975, Neurology.

[93]  Arthur L. Benton,et al.  Differential behavioral effects in frontal lobe disease , 1968 .

[94]  D. A. Grant,et al.  A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. , 1948, Journal of experimental psychology.

[95]  Jörn Diedrichsen,et al.  Dissociating Task-set Selection from Task-set Inhibition in the Prefrontal Cortex , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[96]  H. Braak,et al.  Parkinson's disease: premotor clinico-pathological correlations. , 2006, Journal of neural transmission. Supplementum.

[97]  C. Rorden,et al.  Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. , 2000, Behavioural neurology.

[98]  D. Pandya,et al.  Morphological Correlations of Human and Monkey Frontal Lobe , 1996 .

[99]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[100]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[101]  C. Degueldre,et al.  Decrease of frontal metabolism demonstrated by positron emission tomography in a population of healthy elderly volunteers. , 1991, Acta neurologica Belgica.

[102]  Monica Rosselli,et al.  Neuropsychological characteristics of normal aging , 1989 .

[103]  S. Fahn Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale , 1987 .

[104]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[105]  A. Luria Higher Cortical Functions in Man , 1980, Springer US.