Representation of motion boundaries in retinotopic human visual cortical areas

Edges are important in the interpretation of the retinal image. Although luminance edges have been studied extensively, much less is known about how or where the primate visual system detects boundaries defined by differences in surface properties such as texture, motion or binocular disparity. Here we use functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to localize human visual cortical activity related to the processing of one such higher-order edge type: motion boundaries. We describe a robust fMRI signal that is selective for motion segmentation. This boundary-specific signal is present, and retinotopically organized, within early visual areas, beginning in the primary visual cortex (area V1). Surprisingly, it is largely absent from the motion-selective area MT/V5 and far extrastriate visual areas. Changes in the surface velocity defining the motion boundaries affect the strength of the fMRI signal. In parallel psychophysical experiments, the perceptual salience of the boundaries shows a similar dependence on surface velocity. These results demonstrate that information for segmenting scenes by relative motion is represented as early as V1.

[1]  S Ullman,et al.  Parallel and serial processes in motion detection. , 1987, Science.

[2]  R A Andersen,et al.  The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[3]  A. M. Dale,et al.  La vision, une perception subjective : A la (re)découverte de la panoplie d'aires visuelles du cortex , 1996 .

[4]  D M Levi,et al.  The Perceived Strength of Motion-Defined Edges , 1993, Perception.

[5]  A. M. Dale,et al.  Vision as subjective perception , 1996 .

[6]  Henk Spekreijse,et al.  Contour from motion processing occurs in primary visual cortex , 1993, Nature.

[7]  R. Turner,et al.  Dynamic magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activity during primary sensory stimulation. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[8]  G. Orban,et al.  A motion area in human visual cortex. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[9]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Intrinsic signal changes accompanying sensory stimulation: functional brain mapping with magnetic resonance imaging. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[10]  A. Dale,et al.  Improved Localizadon of Cortical Activity by Combining EEG and MEG with MRI Cortical Surface Reconstruction: A Linear Approach , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  R. Born,et al.  Segregation of global and local motion processing in primate middle temporal visual area , 1993, Nature.

[12]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Functional brain mapping by blood oxygenation level-dependent contrast magnetic resonance imaging. A comparison of signal characteristics with a biophysical model. , 1993, Biophysical journal.

[13]  J M Zanker,et al.  Cortical potentials reflecting motion processing in humans , 1994, Visual Neuroscience.

[14]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Modulation of motion aftereffect by surround motion and its dependence on stimulus size and eccentricity , 1995, Vision Research.

[15]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  R. Andersen,et al.  Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[17]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Responses of neurons in macaque MT to stochastic motion signals , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[18]  P. H. Schiller,et al.  The effects of V4 and middle temporal (MT) area lesions on visual performance in the rhesus monkey , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[19]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The connections of the middle temporal visual area (MT) and their relationship to a cortical hierarchy in the macaque monkey , 1983, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[20]  O. Braddick Segmentation versus integration in visual motion processing , 1993, Trends in Neurosciences.

[21]  D. Regan,et al.  Visual processing of motion-defined form: selective failure in patients with parietotemporal lesions , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  G. Orban,et al.  Processing of kinetically defined boundaries in the cortical motion area MT of the macaque monkey. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  G. Orban,et al.  Laminar analysis of motion information processing in macaque V5 , 1989, Brain Research.

[24]  J W Belliveau,et al.  Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1995, Science.

[25]  Eka Swadiansa The hypothesis , 1990 .

[26]  E. DeYoe,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging (FMRI) of the human brain , 1994, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[27]  E. DeYoe,et al.  Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human cerebral cortex. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Optical Velocity Patterns, Velocity-Sensitive Neurons, and Space Perception: A Hypothesis , 1974, Perception.

[29]  O. Braddick A short-range process in apparent motion. , 1974, Vision research.

[30]  R. Kikinis,et al.  Segregation of computations underlying perception of motion discontinuity and coherence. , 1994, Neuroreport.

[31]  Qasim Zaidi,et al.  Visual processing of motion boundaries , 1995, Vision Research.

[32]  Adrian T. Lee,et al.  fMRI of human visual cortex , 1994, Nature.