Imagery of movements immediately following performance allows learning of motor skills that interfere
暂无分享,去创建一个
Daniel M Wolpert | Hannah R Sheahan | James N Ingram | Goda M Žalalytė | D. Wolpert | J. Ingram | Goda Žalalytė | Hannah Sheahan
[1] A. Sirigu,et al. The Mental Representation of Hand Movements After Parietal Cortex Damage , 1996, Science.
[2] J. Kalaska,et al. Covariation of primate dorsal premotor cell activity with direction and amplitude during a memorized-delay reaching task. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.
[3] Rick Grush,et al. The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[4] Chethan Pandarinath,et al. Neural population dynamics in human motor cortex during movements in people with ALS , 2015, eLife.
[5] R. Shadmehr,et al. A Gain-Field Encoding of Limb Position and Velocity in the Internal Model of Arm Dynamics , 2003, PLoS biology.
[6] P. Jackson,et al. The neural network of motor imagery: An ALE meta-analysis , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[7] J. Randall Flanagan,et al. Multiple motor memories are learned to control different points on a tool , 2018, Nature Human Behaviour.
[8] Matthew T. Kaufman,et al. Neural population dynamics during reaching , 2012, Nature.
[9] Byron M. Yu,et al. Neural Variability in Premotor Cortex Provides a Signature of Motor Preparation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[10] D. Wolpert,et al. The Value of the Follow-Through Derives from Motor Learning Depending on Future Actions , 2015, Current Biology.
[11] M. Sahani,et al. Cortical control of arm movements: a dynamical systems perspective. , 2013, Annual review of neuroscience.
[12] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[13] Nicolas Schweighofer,et al. Motor learning without doing: trial-by-trial improvement in motor performance during mental training. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.
[14] K. Shenoy,et al. Neural Population Dynamics Underlying Motor Learning Transfer , 2018, Neuron.
[15] Marco Schieppati,et al. Imagined and actual arm movements have similar durations when performed under different conditions of direction and mass , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[16] T. Ebner,et al. Neuronal specification of direction and distance during reaching movements in the superior precentral premotor area and primary motor cortex of monkeys. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.
[17] A. Riehle,et al. Monkey primary motor and premotor cortex: single-cell activity related to prior information about direction and extent of an intended movement. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.
[18] R. Ivry,et al. The Neural Specificity of Movement Preparation During Actual and Imagined Movements , 2019, Cerebral cortex.
[19] D. Wolpert,et al. The effect of contextual cues on the encoding of motor memories , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.
[20] Lorna M. Lopez,et al. Modulation of Genetic Associations with Serum Urate Levels by Body-Mass-Index in Humans , 2015, PloS one.
[21] Pavel Bobrov,et al. Increased motor cortex excitability during motor imagery in brain-computer interface trained subjects , 2013, Front. Comput. Neurosci..
[22] T. Pozzo,et al. Improvement and generalization of arm motor performance through motor imagery practice , 2006, Neuroscience.
[23] S. Wolf,et al. The Movement Imagery Questionnaire-Revised, Second Edition (MIQ-RS) Is a Reliable and Valid Tool for Evaluating Motor Imagery in Stroke Populations , 2012, Evidence-based complementary and alternative medicine : eCAM.
[24] R A Scheidt,et al. Persistence of motor adaptation during constrained, multi-joint, arm movements. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.
[25] P. Dechent,et al. Is the human primary motor cortex involved in motor imagery? , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[26] Stephen I. Ryu,et al. Neural Dynamics of Reaching following Incorrect or Absent Motor Preparation , 2014, Neuron.
[27] Daniel M. Wolpert,et al. A modular planar robotic manipulandum with end-point torque control , 2009, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[28] R. Johansson,et al. Prediction Precedes Control in Motor Learning , 2003, Current Biology.
[29] R. Gutierrez,et al. Inhibition of Advanced Glycation End-Product Formation byOriganum majoranaL.In Vitroand in Streptozotocin-Induced Diabetic Rats , 2012 .
[30] S. Wise,et al. Effects of hand movement path on motor cortical activity in awake, behaving rhesus monkeys , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[31] David W. Franklin,et al. Neural Tuning Functions Underlie Both Generalization and Interference , 2015, PloS one.
[32] R. Schekman. Editorial Expression of Concern: Use of combinatorial genetic libraries to humanize N-linked glycosylation in the yeast Pichiapastoris , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[33] Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al. Cortical activity during motor execution, motor imagery, and imagery-based online feedback , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[34] J. Decety. Do imagined and executed actions share the same neural substrate? , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[35] R. Gentili,et al. Laterality effects in motor learning by mental practice in right-handers , 2015, Neuroscience.
[36] D. Wolpert,et al. Gone in 0.6 Seconds: The Encoding of Motor Memories Depends on Recent Sensorimotor States , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[37] Craig Hall,et al. The MIQ-RS: A Suitable Option for Examining Movement Imagery Ability , 2007, Evidence-based complementary and alternative medicine : eCAM.
[38] Simon B. Eickhoff,et al. Neural Correlates of Motor Imagery, Action Observation, and Movement Execution: A Comparison Across Quantitative Meta-Analyses , 2017, bioRxiv.
[39] M. Jeannerod,et al. The timing of mentally represented actions , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.
[40] Carolyn Copper,et al. Does mental practice enhance performance , 1994 .
[41] Paul Maruff,et al. The effect of an external load on the force and timing components of mentally represented actions , 2000, Behavioural Brain Research.
[42] T. Kasai,et al. Evidence for facilitation of motor evoked potentials (MEPs) induced by motor imagery , 1997, Brain Research.
[43] D. Nozaki,et al. Distinct Motor Plans Form and Retrieve Distinct Motor Memories for Physically Identical Movements , 2012, Current Biology.
[44] F. P. de Lange,et al. Motor imagery of gait: a quantitative approach , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[45] David W Franklin,et al. Impedance control and internal model use during the initial stage of adaptation to novel dynamics in humans , 2005, The Journal of physiology.
[46] D. Wolpert,et al. Motor Planning, Not Execution, Separates Motor Memories , 2016, Neuron.
[47] M. Jeannerod. Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition , 2001, NeuroImage.
[48] S. Swinnen,et al. Kinesthetic, but not visual, motor imagery modulates corticomotor excitability , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[49] R. Shackleton. A Quantitative Approach , 2005 .
[50] E. Bizzi,et al. Consolidation in human motor memory , 1996, Nature.
[51] E Bizzi,et al. Motor learning by field approximation. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[52] Gopal Santhanam,et al. Preparatory activity in premotor and motor cortex reflects the speed of the upcoming reach. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[53] Charalambos Papaxanthis,et al. Neural plasticity during motor learning with motor imagery practice: Review and perspectives , 2017, Neuroscience.
[54] M. Jeannerod,et al. Mentally simulated movements in virtual reality: does Fitt's law hold in motor imagery? , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.