Where do relative specifiers come from?

This paper argues that the emergence of dependent wh-relatives in Middle English was an instance of diffusion of a novel syntactic property through the set of wh-forms, construed as a series of reanalyses of individual lexical items. This seemingly parochial claim takes on a broader significance in the context of two well-established findings about the typology of relativization. First, similar constructions (dependent relatives containing a filled [Spec,CP], or relative specifier) have an unusual typological distribution: they are largely clustered in Indo-European languages, but probably not present in Proto-Indo-European. This leads us to ask where relative specifiers come from, or more broadly, how daughter languages can share an otherwise rare property which they did not inherit from a common ancestor. Second, the research tradition stemming from Keenan and Comrie (1977) associates relative specifiers with positions low on the Accessibility Hierarchy. Romaine (1980, 1982, 1984) describes the genesis of English dependent wh-relatives in just those terms: they were initially confined to low-accessibility functions, and subsequently spread up the hierarchy. This pattern is very common among languages with relative specifiers (Hendery 2012), and may be a diachronic universal. These findings invite analyses based on two tenets: (1) a near-complementarity between the initial distribution of dependent wh-relatives and that-relatives, the primary relativization strategy in Keenan and Comrie’s terms, which suggests that whrelatives emerged to replace the earlier demonstrative series of relativizers in functions which could not be relativized by that (Romaine 1982:450); (2) work by Keenan and Hawkins (1987), Hawkins (1995), and Kirby (1996) grounding the Accessibility Hierarchy in processing factors, which suggests that patterns described in the terms of the Accessibility Hierarchy should have explanations in those terms. These functionalist analyses ask “in Language L at time t, which forms were available for which types of relativizaton?”. This approach leads to a surprising position, where formally distinct elements (such as demonstratives and interrogatives) are considered part of the same system, provided that they do the same job. Accordingly, we find quotes like this:

[1]  A. Meillet,et al.  Introduction à l'étude comparative des langues indoeuropéennes , 1922 .

[2]  Lucien Tesnière Éléments de syntaxe structurale , 1959 .

[3]  Tauno F. Mustanoja,et al.  A Middle English syntax , 1960 .

[4]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  वाक्यविन्यास का सैद्धान्तिक पक्ष = Aspects of the theory of syntax , 1965 .

[5]  Samuel Jay Keyser,et al.  A Partial History of the Relative Clause in English , 1975 .

[6]  C. Allen,et al.  Topics in diachronic English syntax , 1977 .

[7]  Edward L. Keenan,et al.  Noun Phrase Accessibility Revisited , 1979 .

[8]  Dan Maxwell,et al.  Strategies of Relativization and NP Accessibility , 1979 .

[9]  Suzanne Romaine,et al.  The relative clause marker in Scots English: Diffusion, complexity, and style as dimensions of syntactic change , 1980, Language in Society.

[10]  Suzanne Romaine Socio-Historical Linguistics: Its Status and Methodology , 1982 .

[11]  Dan Maxwell IMPLICATIONS OF NP ACCESSIBILITY FOR DIACHRONIC SYNTAX , 1982 .

[12]  Suzanne Romaine,et al.  Towards a typology of relative clause formation strategies in Germanic , 1984 .

[13]  D. Slobin Crosslinguistic Evidence for the Language-making Capacity , 1985 .

[14]  E. Clark On the logic of contrast. , 1988, Journal of child language.

[15]  E. Markman,et al.  Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  A. Kroch Reflexes of grammar in patterns of language change , 1989, Language Variation and Change.

[17]  Veneeta Srivastav,et al.  The syntax and semantics of correlatives , 1991 .

[18]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  The Minimalist Program , 1992 .

[19]  John A. Hawkins,et al.  A Performance Theory of Order and Constituency , 1995 .

[20]  J. Elman,et al.  Learning and morphological change , 1995, Cognition.

[21]  Daniel Gildea,et al.  Learning Bias and Phonological-Rule Induction , 1996, CL.

[22]  A. Kroch Morphosyntactic Variation , 1997 .

[23]  Bernard Comrie,et al.  Rethinking the Typology of Relative Clauses , 1998 .

[24]  Simon Kirby,et al.  Function, Selection, and Innateness: The Emergence of Language Universals , 1999 .

[25]  de Mark Vries,et al.  The Syntax of Relativization , 2002 .

[26]  H. Hughes The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language , 2003 .

[27]  B. Hayes,et al.  Rules vs. analogy in English past tenses: a computational/experimental study , 2003, Cognition.

[28]  Susan Pintzuk,et al.  The York-Toronto-Helsinki Parsed Corpus of Old English , 2003 .

[29]  Mark Steedman,et al.  The syntactic process , 2004, Language, speech, and communication.

[30]  M. Vries,et al.  The Syntax of Appositive Relativization: On Specifying Coordination, False Free Relatives, and Promotion , 2006, Linguistic Inquiry.

[31]  James Clackson,et al.  Indo-European Linguistics , 2007 .

[32]  E. Keenan,et al.  Noun Phrase Accessibility and Universal Grammar , 2008 .

[33]  Adam Albright,et al.  Modeling analogy as probabilistic grammar , 2008 .

[34]  昌明 神谷,et al.  中英語に現れる小節・結果構文-Penn-Helsinki Parsed Corpus of Middle English Second Editionを検索して- , 2010 .

[35]  I. Sag Sign-Based Construction Grammar: An Informal Synopsis , 2012 .

[36]  Rachel Hendery Relative Clauses in Time and Space: A Case Study in the Methods of Diachronic Typology , 2012 .

[37]  R. Truswell,et al.  Quantificational variability and the genesis of English headed wh-relatives , 2015 .

[38]  Franziska Frankfurter,et al.  Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure: Adele E. Goldberg, Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press, 1995. xi + 265 pp , 1998 .