Mechanism for a sliding synaptic modification threshold

Long-term potentiation (LTP) is a long-lasting increase in synaptic effectiveness that results from high frequency stimulation (HFS) of certain excitatory synapses in hippo-campus and neocortex (reviewed by Bliss and Colling-ridge, 1993). Because it is induced by brief episodes of electrical stimulation that resemble patterns observed in the brains of behaving animals, and because it can be very long-lasting, LTP has attracted a great deal of interest as a possible synaptic mechanism for learning and memory. In addition to this role, it has been suggested that the properties of LTP are also well-suited to account for aspects of experience-dependent development. LTP results when presynaptic activity coincides with strong post-synaptic depolarization beyond a threshold value. Thus, the development of binocularly responsive neurons in the visual cortex, for example, could be accounted for by asso-ciative LTP caused by the simultaneous activation of converging inputs from the two eyes. In addition to an LTP-like mechanism, it has been recognized for some time that, to account for developmental plasticity in the cortex, there must be a mechanism for activity-dependent decreases in synaptic strength. For example , during acritical period of early postnatal life, depriving an eye of visual patterns leads to an activity-dependent decrease in the effectiveness of deprived synapses in visual cortex. Indeed, the activity-dependent pruning of connections is a very prominent feature of postnatal development throughout the nervous system. Thus, developmental neurobiologists have sought an experimental model for use-dependent decreases in synaptic strength in the CNS. Here, theoretical analysis of developmental plasticity has been of considerable value. According to the Bienenstock-Cooper-Munro (BCM) theory developed to account for aspects of visual cortical development, long-term depression (LTD) should result when input activity consistently fails to activate postsynaptic neurons beyond the threshold for LTP (Bienenstock et al., 1982; Bear et al., 1987). This insight inspired the attempt to produce LTD with repetitive low frequency stimulation (LFS; Dudek and Bear, 1992). LFS-induced LTD is now a well-established model of synaptic plasticity in slices of hippo-campus and neocortex (reviewed by Bear and Malenka, 1994). In the CA1 region of hippocampus and in layer Ill of visual cortex, LFS of excitatory afferents produces LTD, and HFS produces LTP (Kirkwood et al., 1993). Both forms of synaptic plasticity depend on N-methyl-o-aspartate (NMDA) receptor activation and postsynaptic Ca " + entry. Available data support a model in which the state of correlation of pre-and postsynaptic aciiviiy is converted 5y the voltage-dependent …

[1]  Jian Wang,et al.  CaMKII regulates the frequency-response function of hippocampal synapses for the production of both LTD and LTP , 1995, Cell.

[2]  M. Bear,et al.  Synaptic plasticity: LTP and LTD , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[3]  G. Carmignoto,et al.  Activity-dependent decrease in NMDA receptor responses during development of the visual cortex. , 1992, Science.

[4]  Hiroshi Kato,et al.  Reversal of long-term potentiation (depotentiation) induced by tetanus stimulation of the input to CA1 neurons of guinea pig hippocampal slices , 1991, Brain Research.

[5]  E. Bienenstock,et al.  Theory for the development of neuron selectivity: orientation specificity and binocular interaction in visual cortex , 1982, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[6]  T. Bliss,et al.  A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus , 1993, Nature.

[7]  SM Dudek,et al.  Bidirectional long-term modification of synaptic effectiveness in the adult and immature hippocampus , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  D. Muller,et al.  Long-term potentiation is associated with an increased activity of Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II. , 1993, The Journal of biological chemistry.

[9]  R. Malenka,et al.  Involvement of a calcineurin/ inhibitor-1 phosphatase cascade in hippocampal long-term depression , 1994, Nature.

[10]  P. Stanton,et al.  Priming of homosynaptic long-term depression in hippocampus by previous synaptic activity. , 1993, Neuroreport.

[11]  B. Alger,et al.  GABAergic and developmental influences on homosynaptic LTD and depotentiation in rat hippocampus , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  L. Cooper,et al.  A physiological basis for a theory of synapse modification. , 1987, Science.

[13]  R. Malinow,et al.  Potentiated transmission and prevention of further LTP by increased CaMKII activity in postsynaptic hippocampal slice neurons. , 1994, Science.

[14]  H. Schulman,et al.  Neuronal Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinases. , 1992, Annual review of biochemistry.

[15]  E. Kandel,et al.  Impairment of spatial but not contextual memory in CaMKII mutant mice with a selective loss of hippocampal ltp in the range of the θ frequency , 1995, Cell.

[16]  M. Bear,et al.  Common forms of synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus and neocortex in vitro. , 1993, Science.

[17]  J. Lisman The CaM kinase II hypothesis for the storage of synaptic memory , 1994, Trends in Neurosciences.

[18]  R. Nicoll,et al.  An essential role for postsynaptic calmodulin and protein kinase activity in long-term potentiation , 1989, Nature.

[19]  Alcino J. Silva,et al.  Deficient hippocampal long-term potentiation in alpha-calcium-calmodulin kinase II mutant mice. , 1992, Science.

[20]  R. Malenka,et al.  The influence of prior synaptic activity on the induction of long-term potentiation. , 1992, Science.