Temporal and spectral profiles of stimulus–stimulus and stimulus–response conflict processing

The ability to detect and resolve conflict is an essential function of cognitive control. Laboratory studies often use stimulus-response-compatibility (SRC) tasks to examine conflict processing in order to elucidate the mechanism and modular organization of cognitive control. Inspired by two influential theories regarding cognitive control, the conflict monitoring theory (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001) and dimensional overlap taxonomy (Kornblum, Hasbroucq, & Osman, 1990), we explored the temporal and spectral similarities and differences between processing of stimulus-stimulus (S-S) and stimulus-response (S-R) conflicts with event related potential (ERP) and time-frequency measures. We predicted that processing of S-S conflict starts earlier than that of S-R conflict and that the two types of conflict may involve different frequency bands. Participants were asked to perform two parallel SRC tasks, both combining the Stroop task (involving S-S conflict) and Simon task (involving S-R conflict). ERP results showed pronounced SRC effects (incongruent vs. congruent) on N2 and P3 components for both S-S and S-R conflicts. In both tasks, SRC effects of S-S conflict took place earlier than those of S-R conflict. Time-frequency analysis revealed that both types of SRC effects modulated theta and alpha bands, while S-R conflict effects additionally modulated power in the beta band. These results indicated that although S-S and S-R conflict processing shared considerable ERP and time-frequency properties, they differed in temporal and spectral dynamics. We suggest that the modular organization of cognitive control should take both commonality and distinction of S-S and S-R conflict processing into consideration.

[1]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Interactions between stimulus-stimulus congruence and stimulus-response compatibility , 1997 .

[2]  Jin Fan,et al.  Dimensional overlap accounts for independence and integration of stimulus—response compatibility effects , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[3]  J. R. Simon,et al.  Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. , 1967, The Journal of applied psychology.

[4]  C Alain,et al.  Event-related neural activity associated with the Stroop task. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[5]  C. Carter,et al.  The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[7]  C. Carter,et al.  The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies , 2002, Physiology & Behavior.

[8]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Evidence for distinct beta resonance frequencies in human EEG related to specific sensorimotor cortical areas , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[9]  Dennis Van 't Ent Perceptual and motor contributions to performance and ERP components after incorrect motor activation in a flanker reaction task. , 2002, Clinical neurophysiology : official journal of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology.

[10]  W. Klimesch,et al.  The functional significance of theta and upper alpha oscillations. , 2005, Experimental psychology.

[11]  J. Requin,et al.  The Effects of Irrelevant Stimuli: 1. The Time Course of Stimulus-Stimulus and Stimulus-Response Consistency Effects With Stroop-Like Stimuli, Simon-Like Tasks, and Their Factorial Combinations , 1999 .

[12]  John Polich,et al.  P300 and response time from a manual Stroop task , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[13]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  Response Anticipation and Response Conflict: An Event-Related Potential and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  T. Egner,et al.  Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  R. Nigbur,et al.  Theta power as a marker for cognitive interference , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[16]  G. D. Logan Task Switching , 2022 .

[17]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[18]  Hartmut Leuthold,et al.  The Simon effect in cognitive electrophysiology: A short review. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[19]  E. Lauber,et al.  Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  S. Baker Oscillatory interactions between sensorimotor cortex and the periphery , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[21]  S. Bressler,et al.  Response preparation and inhibition: The role of the cortical sensorimotor beta rhythm , 2008, Neuroscience.

[22]  W. Klimesch,et al.  Control mechanisms in working memory: A possible function of EEG theta oscillations , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[23]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  J R Simon,et al.  Effect of conflicting cues on information processing: the 'Stroop effect' vs. the 'Simon effect'. , 1990, Acta psychologica.

[25]  Erwin Hennighausen,et al.  N200 in the Eriksen-Task: Inhibitory Executive Processes? , 2000 .

[26]  Simon Hanslmayr,et al.  The Electrophysiological Dynamics of Interference during the Stroop Task , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[28]  J. Lisman,et al.  Oscillations in the alpha band (9-12 Hz) increase with memory load during retention in a short-term memory task. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[30]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[31]  S Kornblum,et al.  The way irrelevant dimensions are processed depends on what they overlap with: The case of Stroop- and Simon-like stimuli , 1994, Psychological research.

[32]  Carlo Umiltà,et al.  Flanker and Simon effects interact at the response selection stage , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[33]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[34]  Manfred Herrmann,et al.  Spatio-temporal brain dynamics in a combined stimulus–stimulus and stimulus–response conflict task , 2011, NeuroImage.

[35]  J. Jonides,et al.  Interference resolution: Insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[36]  D. Tucker,et al.  Frontal midline theta and the error-related negativity: neurophysiological mechanisms of action regulation , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[37]  T. Egner Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  W. Klimesch,et al.  Relevance of EEG alpha and theta oscillations during task switching , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  D. Mathalon,et al.  Event-related EEG time-frequency analysis: an overview of measures and an analysis of early gamma band phase locking in schizophrenia. , 2008, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[40]  Xun Liu,et al.  Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial Stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI , 2004, NeuroImage.

[41]  B. Kopp,et al.  N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[42]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Prelude to and Resolution of an Error: EEG Phase Synchrony Reveals Cognitive Control Dynamics during Action Monitoring , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  S. Wiens,et al.  Behavioral and ERP indices of response conflict in Stroop and flanker tasks. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[44]  E. J. Stoffels,et al.  Effects of visual and auditory noise on visual choice reaction time in a continuous-flow paradigm , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[45]  Jonathan R. Folstein,et al.  Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[46]  Sylvan Kornblum,et al.  Distributional Analysis and De Jong, Liang, and Lauber's (1994) Dual-Process Model of the Simon Effect , 1997 .

[47]  H. Mayberg,et al.  An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color-word interference effect , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Theta Dynamics Reveal Domain-specific Control over Stimulus and Response Conflict , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[49]  F. Valle-Inclán,et al.  The locus of interference in the Simon effect: an ERP study , 1996, Biological Psychology.

[50]  S. Makeig,et al.  Mining event-related brain dynamics , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[51]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.