Rostral and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex make dissociable contributions during antisaccade error commission

The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) participates in both performance optimization and evaluation, with dissociable contributions from dorsal (dACC) and rostral (rACC) regions. Deactivation in rACC and other default-mode regions is important for performance optimization, whereas increased rACC and dACC activation contributes to performance evaluation. Errors activate both rACC and dACC. We propose that this activation reflects differential error-related involvement of rACC and dACC during both performance optimization and evaluation, and that these two processes can be distinguished by the timing of their occurrence within a trial. We compared correct and error antisaccade trials. We expected errors to correlate with an early failure of rACC deactivation and increased activation of both rACC and dACC later in the trial. Eighteen healthy subjects performed a series of prosaccade and antisaccade trials during event-related functional MRI. We estimated the hemodynamic responses for error and correct antisaccades using a finite impulse-response model. We examined ACC activity by comparing error and correct antisaccades with a fixation baseline and error to correct antisaccades directly. Compared with correct antisaccades, errors were characterized by an early bilateral failure of deactivation of rACC and other default-mode regions. This difference was significant in rACC. Errors also were associated with increased activity in both rACC and dACC later in the trial. These results show that accurate performance involves deactivation of the rACC and other default mode regions and suggest that both rACC and dACC contribute to the evaluation of error responses.

[1]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[2]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[3]  M. Posner,et al.  Localization of a Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1994 .

[4]  N. Alpert,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow measured during symptom provocation in obsessive-compulsive disorder using oxygen 15-labeled carbon dioxide and positron emission tomography. , 1994, Archives of general psychiatry.

[5]  S. Schachter,et al.  Ambilaterality: definition from handedness preference questionnaires and potential significance. , 1994, The International journal of neuroscience.

[6]  J W Belliveau,et al.  Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1995, Science.

[7]  B. Vogt,et al.  Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Improved Assessment of Significant Activation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI): Use of a Cluster‐Size Threshold , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[9]  M. Mintun,et al.  Positron emission tomography study of voluntary saccadic eye movements and spatial working memory. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  D. Heeger,et al.  Linear Systems Analysis of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Human V1 , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  A. Dale,et al.  Selective averaging of rapidly presented individual trials using fMRI , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[13]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[14]  George Bush,et al.  The emotional counting stroop paradigm: a functional magnetic resonance imaging probe of the anterior cingulate affective division , 1998, Biological Psychiatry.

[15]  Marcus E. Raichle,et al.  Suppression of Regional Cerebral Blood during Emotional versus Higher Cognitive Implications for Interactions between Emotion and Cognition , 1998 .

[16]  S. Rauch,et al.  The counting stroop: An interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging—validation study with functional MRI , 1998, Human brain mapping.

[17]  J. A. Frost,et al.  Conceptual Processing during the Conscious Resting State: A Functional MRI Study , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  R W Cox,et al.  Real‐time 3D image registration for functional MRI , 1999, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[19]  L. Leach,et al.  Neuropsychological Differential Diagnosis , 1999 .

[20]  A. Dale,et al.  Cortical Surface-Based Analysis II: Inflation, Flattening, and a Surface-Based Coordinate System , 1999, NeuroImage.

[21]  A M Dale,et al.  Optimal experimental design for event‐related fMRI , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[22]  E. Halgren,et al.  Dynamic Statistical Parametric Mapping Combining fMRI and MEG for High-Resolution Imaging of Cortical Activity , 2000, Neuron.

[23]  D. Tucker,et al.  Mood, personality, and self-monitoring: negative affect and emotionality in relation to frontal lobe mechanisms of error monitoring. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[24]  A M Dale,et al.  Estimation and detection of event‐related fMRI signals with temporally correlated noise: A statistically efficient and unbiased approach , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[25]  K. Kiehl,et al.  Error processing and the rostral anterior cingulate: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[26]  W. Drevets Neuroimaging studies of mood disorders , 2000, Biological Psychiatry.

[27]  S Thesen,et al.  Prospective acquisition correction for head motion with image‐based tracking for real‐time fMRI , 2000, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[28]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  M. Raichle,et al.  Emotion-induced changes in human medial prefrontal cortex: I. During cognitive task performance. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[30]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[31]  D. Wilkin,et al.  Neuron , 2001, Brain Research.

[32]  Craig E. L. Stark,et al.  When zero is not zero: The problem of ambiguous baseline conditions in fMRI , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  R. Savoy History and future directions of human brain mapping and functional neuroimaging. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[34]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: evidence from an antisaccade task. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[35]  G. Glover,et al.  Error‐related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[36]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Automated manifold surgery: constructing geometrically accurate and topologically correct models of the human cerebral cortex , 2001, IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging.

[37]  A. Dale,et al.  Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: A role in reward-based decision making , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  M. Raichle,et al.  Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[39]  Thomas Elbert,et al.  Dissociation in human prefrontal cortex of affective influences on working memory-related activity , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[40]  Gregory V. Simpson,et al.  Evidence for Anterior Cingulate Cortex Involvement in Monitoring Preparatory Attentional Set , 2002, NeuroImage.

[41]  C. Carter,et al.  The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[43]  Mariya V. Cherkasova,et al.  Schizophrenic subjects show deficient inhibition but intact task switching on saccadic tasks , 2002, Biological Psychiatry.

[44]  H Garavan,et al.  A midline dissociation between error-processing and response-conflict monitoring , 2003, NeuroImage.

[45]  J. Binder,et al.  A Parametric Manipulation of Factors Affecting Task-induced Deactivation in Functional Neuroimaging , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  R. Simons,et al.  To err is autonomic: error-related brain potentials, ANS activity, and post-error compensatory behavior. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[47]  D. Tucker,et al.  Electrophysiological Responses to Errors and Feedback in the Process of Action Regulation , 2003, Psychological science.

[48]  C. Curtis,et al.  Success and Failure Suppressing Reflexive Behavior , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[49]  S. Rauch,et al.  Neurobiology of emotion perception I: the neural basis of normal emotion perception , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[50]  Hugh Garavan,et al.  Individual differences in error processing: a review and reanalysis of three event-related fMRI studies using the GO/NOGO task. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[51]  D. V. Cramon,et al.  Decision making, performance and outcome monitoring in frontal cortical areas , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[52]  R. Simons,et al.  Error-related psychophysiology and negative affect , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[53]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Predicting Success: Patterns of Cortical Activation and Deactivation Prior to Response Inhibition , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[54]  Nikos Makris,et al.  Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[55]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  Neuroimaging of Performance Monitoring: Error Detection and Beyond , 2004, Cortex.