A dual-task paradigm to study the interference reduction in the Simon task.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Franck Vidal | Laurence Casini | Boris Burle | Isabel Suarez | B. Burle | F. Vidal | L. Casini | I. Suarez
[1] K Richard Ridderinkhof,et al. Delta plots in the study of individual differences: new tools reveal response inhibition deficits in AD/Hd that are eliminated by methylphenidate treatment. , 2005, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[2] F. Vidal,et al. Programming response duration in a precueing reaction time paradigm. , 1991, Journal of motor behavior.
[3] F. Macar,et al. Effects of attention manipulation on judgments of duration and of intensity in the visual modality , 1997, Memory & cognition.
[4] B. Hommel. Spontaneous decay of response-code activation , 1994, Psychological research.
[5] Scott W. Brown. Attentional resources in timing: Interference effects in concurrent temporal and nontemporal working memory tasks , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[6] Eliot Hazeltine,et al. Simultaneous dual-task performance reveals parallel response selection after practice. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] Jennifer M. Glass,et al. Virtually Perfect Time Sharing in Dual-Task Performance: Uncorking the Central Cognitive Bottleneck , 2001, Psychological science.
[8] J. Müsseler,et al. Processing of irrelevant location information under dual-task conditions , 2006, Psychological research.
[9] R. Hübner,et al. Evidence for strategic suppression of irrelevant activation in the Simon task. , 2013, Acta psychologica.
[10] B. Hommel. The Simon effect as tool and heuristic. , 2011, Acta psychologica.
[11] C D Wickens,et al. Compatibility and Resource Competition between Modalities of Input, Central Processing, and Output , 1983, Human factors.
[12] R W Proctor,et al. Referential coding and attention-shifting accounts of the Simon effect , 1994, Psychological research.
[13] Harold Pashler,et al. Do response modality effects support multiprocessor models of divided attention , 1990 .
[14] J. Müsseler,et al. Testing the boundary conditions for processing irrelevant location information: The cross-task Simon effect , 2005 .
[15] Borís Burle,et al. Executive control in the Simon effect: an electromyographic and distributional analysis , 2002, Psychological research.
[16] Wolf Schwarz,et al. Response time models of delta plots with negative-going slopes , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[17] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: Empirical clarification through distributional analyses. , 2002 .
[18] Borís Burle,et al. Dynamics of facilitation and interference in cue-priming and Simon tasks , 2005 .
[19] R. E. Hicks,et al. Concurrent processing demands and the experience of time-in-passing , 1977 .
[20] F. Macar,et al. Controlled attention sharing influences time estimation , 1994, Memory & cognition.
[21] H Pashler,et al. Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[22] Scott D. Brown,et al. On the linear relation between the mean and the standard deviation of a response time distribution. , 2007, Psychological review.
[23] B Burle,et al. Dissociation between activation and attention effects in time estimation: implications for internal clock models. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[24] Ewart A. C. Thomas,et al. Cognitive processing and time perception , 1975 .
[25] S Kornblum,et al. The way irrelevant dimensions are processed depends on what they overlap with: The case of Stroop- and Simon-like stimuli , 1994, Psychological research.
[26] Huifang Wang,et al. Activation of response codes by relevant and irrelevant stimulus information , 1995 .
[27] S. B. Vincent. The function of the vibrissae in the behavior of the white rat , 1912 .
[28] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Subthalamic nucleus stimulation influences expression and suppression of impulsive behaviour in Parkinson's disease. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[29] Richard Ridderinkhof. Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: activation and suppression in conflict tasks , 2002, Psychological research.
[30] R. Ratcliff. Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.
[31] E. Lauber,et al. Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[32] Daniel Gopher,et al. On the Economy of the Human Processing System: A Model of Multiple Capacity. , 1977 .
[33] Christopher D. Wickens,et al. The Structure of Attentional Resources , 1980 .
[34] A. Hedge,et al. The effect of irrelevant spatial correspondences on two-choice response-time. , 1975, Acta psychologica.
[35] P. Hancock,et al. How cognitive load affects duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[36] J. R. Simon,et al. Reactions toward the source of stimulation. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.
[37] Peter C. M. Molenaar,et al. On the relation between the mean and the variance of a diffusion model response time distribution , 2005 .
[38] D. Navon,et al. Role of outcome conflict in dual-task interference. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[39] B. J. Winer. Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .
[40] J. R. Simon. The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional CUE on Human Information Processing , 1990 .
[41] E. Soetens,et al. Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[42] R. Remington,et al. The role of input and output modality pairings in dual-task performance: Evidence for content-dependent central interference , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.