Cultural evolution of conformity and anticonformity

Significance The evolutionary dynamics of cultural variants under conformist- and anticonformist-biased transmission have implications for humans and nonhuman animals. Humans display both conformist and anticonformist biases, and models of conformist-biased transmission have been proposed to explain large-scale human cooperation. Nonhuman animals have been shown to display conformist biases in mating and foraging decisions. Here, we investigate established mathematical models of conformist and anticonformist bias with and without selection and find complex dynamics, including multiple stable polymorphic equilibria, stable cycles, and chaos. Using modifier theory, we show that evolution will reduce anticonformist bias against a culturally advantageous variant. Migration between subpopulations subject to different selection and conformity pressures can produce interesting polymorphisms or eliminate between-group differences. Conformist bias occurs when the probability of adopting a more common cultural variant in a population exceeds its frequency, and anticonformist bias occurs when the reverse is true. Conformist and anticonformist bias have been widely documented in humans, and conformist bias has also been observed in many nonhuman animals. Boyd and Richerson used models of conformist and anticonformist bias to explain the evolution of large-scale cooperation, and subsequent research has extended these models. We revisit Boyd and Richerson’s original analysis and show that, with conformity based on more than three role models, the evolutionary dynamics can be more complex than previously assumed. For example, we show the presence of stable cycles and chaos under strong anticonformity and the presence of new equilibria when both conformity and anticonformity act at different variant frequencies, with and without selection. We also investigate the case of population subdivision with migration and find that the common claim that conformity can maintain between-group differences is not always true. Therefore, the effect of conformity on the evolution of cooperation by group selection may be more complicated than previously stated. Finally, using Feldman and Liberman’s modifier approach, we investigate the conditions under which a rare modifier of the extent of conformity or the number of role models can invade a population. Understanding the dynamics of conformist- and anticonformist-biased transmission may have implications for research on human and nonhuman animal behavior, the evolution of cooperation, and frequency-dependent transmission in general.

[1]  Kevin N. Laland,et al.  The role of conformity in foraging when personal and social information conflict , 2004 .

[2]  Robert Boyd,et al.  Rapid cultural adaptation can facilitate the evolution of large-scale cooperation , 2010, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[3]  M W Feldman,et al.  An evolutionary reduction principle for genetic modifiers. , 1986, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  K. Sznajd-Weron,et al.  The Hunt Opinion Model—An Agent Based Approach to Recurring Fashion Cycles , 2016, PloS one.

[5]  A. Whiten,et al.  Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees , 2005, Nature.

[6]  Kevin N. Laland,et al.  Conformist learning in nine-spined sticklebacks' foraging decisions , 2010, Biology Letters.

[7]  Mark Lubell,et al.  Conformists and mavericks: the empirics of frequency-dependent cultural transmission , 2008 .

[8]  R. Alexander Bentley,et al.  Biases in cultural transmission shape the turnover of popular traits , 2014 .

[9]  S. Karlin Selection-Migration Structures and Conditions for a Protected Polymorphism , 2010 .

[10]  L. Altenberg,et al.  Unified reduction principle for the evolution of mutation, migration, and recombination , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[11]  D. Haun,et al.  Conformity and its look-a-likes , 2015, Animal Behaviour.

[12]  K. Laland,et al.  Who follows whom? Shoaling preferences and social learning of foraging information in guppies , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[13]  E. Ising Beitrag zur Theorie des Ferromagnetismus , 1925 .

[14]  Luc-Alain Giraldeau,et al.  The evolution of social learning rules: payoff-biased and frequency-dependent biased transmission. , 2009, Journal of theoretical biology.

[15]  P. Richerson,et al.  Culture and the Evolutionary Process , 1988 .

[16]  J. Henrich Cultural group selection, coevolutionary processes and large-scale cooperation , 2004 .

[17]  A. Mesoudi Migration, acculturation, and the maintenance of between-group cultural variation , 2017, bioRxiv.

[18]  Wataru Nakahashi,et al.  The evolution of conformist transmission in social learning when the environment changes periodically. , 2007, Theoretical population biology.

[19]  Katarzyna Sznajd-Weron,et al.  Is Independence Necessary for a Discontinuous Phase Transition within the q-Voter Model? , 2019, Entropy.

[20]  E. Rogers Diffusion of Innovations , 1962 .

[21]  Caroline E Walters,et al.  An SIS model for cultural trait transmission with conformity bias. , 2013, Theoretical population biology.

[22]  D. Haun,et al.  Conformity in nonhuman primates: fad or fact? , 2013 .

[23]  L L Cavalli-Sforza,et al.  Theory and observation in cultural transmission. , 1982, Science.

[24]  R. Boyd,et al.  On Modeling Cognition and Culture: Why cultural evolution does not require replication of representations , 2002 .

[25]  Joseph Henrich,et al.  Cultural transmission and the diffusion of innovations : adoption dynamics indicate that biased cultural transmission is the predominate force in behavioral change and much of sociocultural evolution , 2001 .

[26]  A. Thornton,et al.  Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds , 2014, Nature.

[27]  R Boyd,et al.  Why people punish defectors. Weak conformist transmission can stabilize costly enforcement of norms in cooperative dilemmas. , 2001, Journal of theoretical biology.

[28]  R. Lewontin Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach , 1982 .

[29]  Katarzyna Sznajd-Weron,et al.  Statistical Physics Of Opinion Formation: Is it a SPOOF? , 2019, Comptes Rendus Physique.

[30]  Joe Yuichiro Wakano,et al.  Do social learning and conformist bias coevolve? Henrich and Boyd revisited. , 2007, Theoretical population biology.

[31]  K. Laland,et al.  The development of adaptive conformity in young children: effects of uncertainty and consensus. , 2015, Developmental science.

[32]  Kevin N Laland,et al.  Tradeoffs between the strength of conformity and number of conformists in variable environments. , 2013, Journal of theoretical biology.

[33]  R. Boyd,et al.  Transmission coupling mechanisms: cultural group selection , 2010, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[34]  R. B. Payne,et al.  Biological and cultural success of song memes in indigo buntings , 1988 .

[35]  T. Kameda,et al.  Cost-benefit analysis of social/cultural learning 1 Cost-Benefit Analysis of Social/Cultural Learning in a Non-Stationary Uncertain Environment: An Evolutionary Simulation and an Experiment with Human Subjects , 2002 .

[36]  E. Danchin,et al.  Cultural flies: Conformist social learning in fruitflies predicts long-lasting mate-choice traditions , 2018, Science.

[37]  C. Palmer,et al.  On Cultural Group Selection , 1995, Current Anthropology.

[38]  Peter J. Richerson,et al.  Cultural transmission and the evolution of cooperative behavior , 1982 .

[39]  Marcus W. Feldman,et al.  Cultural Transmission and Evolution (MPB-16), Volume 16: A Quantitative Approach. (MPB-16) , 1981 .

[40]  Richard McElreath,et al.  Conformity does not perpetuate suboptimal traditions in a wild population of songbirds , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[41]  M. Feldman,et al.  Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative approach. , 1981, Monographs in population biology.

[42]  Michael Muthukrishna,et al.  The when and who of social learning and conformist transmission , 2016 .

[43]  R. Boyd,et al.  The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. , 1998 .

[44]  K. Laland,et al.  The evolutionary basis of human social learning , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[45]  L. Altenberg,et al.  The evolution of frequency-dependent cultural transmission. , 2019, Theoretical population biology.

[46]  M. Feldman,et al.  On the evolution of sex determination and the sex ratio in haplodiploid populations , 1982 .

[47]  E. van de Waal,et al.  Potent Social Learning and Conformity Shape a Wild Primate’s Foraging Decisions , 2013, Science.

[48]  B. Galef,et al.  ‘Conformity’ in Norway rats? , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[49]  Daisuke Nakanishi,et al.  Cost–benefit analysis of social/cultural learning in a nonstationary uncertain environment: An evolutionary simulation and an experiment with human subjects , 2002 .

[50]  K. Sznajd-Weron,et al.  Anticonformity or Independence?—Insights from Statistical Physics , 2013 .

[51]  P. J. B. Slater,et al.  The evolution of conformity-enforcing behaviour in cultural communication systems , 2004, Animal Behaviour.

[52]  Joe Yuichiro Wakano,et al.  The Driving Forces of Cultural Complexity , 2016, Human Nature.