A job interview in the MRI scanner: How does indirectness affect addressees and overhearers?

In using language, people not only exchange information, but also navigate their social world - for example, they can express themselves indirectly to avoid losing face. In this functional magnetic resonance imaging study, we investigated the neural correlates of interpreting face-saving indirect replies, in a situation where participants only overheard the replies as part of a conversation between two other people, as well as in a situation where the participants were directly addressed themselves. We created a fictional job interview context where indirect replies serve as a natural communicative strategy to attenuate one's shortcomings, and asked fMRI participants to either pose scripted questions and receive answers from three putative job candidates (addressee condition) or to listen to someone else interview the same candidates (overhearer condition). In both cases, the need to evaluate the candidate ensured that participants had an active interest in comprehending the replies. Relative to direct replies, face-saving indirect replies increased activation in medial prefrontal cortex, bilateral temporo-parietal junction (TPJ), bilateral inferior frontal gyrus and bilateral middle temporal gyrus, in active overhearers and active addressees alike, with similar effect size, and comparable to findings obtained in an earlier passive listening study (Bašnáková et al., 2014). In contrast, indirectness effects in bilateral anterior insula and pregenual ACC, two regions implicated in emotional salience and empathy, were reliably stronger in addressees than in active overhearers. Our findings indicate that understanding face-saving indirect language requires additional cognitive perspective-taking and other discourse-relevant cognitive processing, to a comparable extent in active overhearers and addressees. Furthermore, they indicate that face-saving indirect language draws upon affective systems more in addressees than in overhearers, presumably because the addressee is the one being managed by a face-saving reply. In all, face-saving indirectness provides a window on the cognitive as well as affect-related neural systems involved in human communication.

[1]  John Hoeks,et al.  Electrophysiological Research on Conversation and Discourse Processing , 2014 .

[2]  Frank Van Overwalle,et al.  Understanding others' actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: A meta-analysis , 2009, NeuroImage.

[3]  S. Levinson,et al.  Roots of Human Sociality , 2006 .

[4]  Peter Hagoort,et al.  Beyond the language given: the neural correlates of inferring speaker meaning. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[5]  M. Tomasello Origins of human communication , 2008 .

[6]  S. Levinson,et al.  Brain Mechanisms Underlying Human Communication , 2009, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[7]  M. Costandi Default Mode Network , 2015 .

[8]  A. Roepstorff,et al.  Say it with flowers! An fMRI study of object mediated communication , 2009, Brain and Language.

[9]  Paul Boersma,et al.  Praat: doing phonetics by computer , 2003 .

[10]  J. Schoffelen,et al.  Neural mechanisms of communicative innovation , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[11]  T. Singer,et al.  The neural basis of empathy. , 2012, Annual review of neuroscience.

[12]  Hae-Jeong Park,et al.  Everyday conversation requires cognitive inference: Neural bases of comprehending implicated meanings in conversations , 2013, NeuroImage.

[13]  L. Aravind,et al.  Integration of Word Meaning and World Knowledge in Language Comprehension , 2022 .

[14]  Paul Boersma,et al.  Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer , 2002 .

[15]  Jérôme Prado,et al.  Neural evidence that utterance-processing entails mentalizing: The case of irony , 2012, NeuroImage.

[16]  Van Berkum,et al.  The neuropragmatics of 'simple' utterance comprehension: An ERP review , 2009 .

[17]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[18]  Gina F. Humphreys,et al.  Fusion and Fission of Cognitive Functions in the Human Parietal Cortex , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[19]  Peter W. Foltz,et al.  An introduction to latent semantic analysis , 1998 .

[20]  Remco J. Renken,et al.  Evidence for bilateral involvement in idiom comprehension: An fMRI study , 2007, NeuroImage.

[21]  Rutvik H. Desai,et al.  The neurobiology of semantic memory , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  S. Cappa,et al.  Idiom comprehension: a prefrontal task? , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  Mante S. Nieuwland,et al.  On sense and reference: Examining the functional neuroanatomy of referential processing , 2007, NeuroImage.

[24]  M. Dapretto,et al.  Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with autism: the role of prosody and context. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  Evelyn C. Ferstl,et al.  What Does the Frontomedian Cortex Contribute to Language Processing: Coherence or Theory of Mind? , 2002, NeuroImage.

[26]  Steven Pinker,et al.  The logic of indirect speech , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[27]  Niall W. Duncan,et al.  Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis , 2011, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[28]  Mohammad Ali Salmani Nodoushan Review of: Sbisà, Marina/Östman, Jan-Ola/Verschueren, Jef (eds.) (2011): Philosophical Perspectives for Pragmatics. Amsterdam/Philadelphia.: John Benjamins. (= Handbook of Pragmatic 10). , 2013 .

[29]  D. Norris,et al.  BOLD contrast sensitivity enhancement and artifact reduction with multiecho EPI: Parallel‐acquired inhomogeneity‐desensitized fMRI , 2006, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[30]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Brain's Default Network , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[31]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Looking at the brains behind figurative language—A quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on metaphor, idiom, and irony processing , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  M. Just,et al.  Differentiable cortical networks for inferences concerning people's intentions versus physical causality , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[33]  T. Landauer,et al.  A Solution to Plato's Problem: The Latent Semantic Analysis Theory of Acquisition, Induction, and Representation of Knowledge. , 1997 .

[34]  P. Hervé,et al.  A Shared Neural Substrate for Mentalizing and the Affective Component of Sentence Comprehension , 2013, PloS one.

[35]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  Cognitive contributions of the ventral parietal cortex: an integrative theoretical account , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  Daniel Casasanto,et al.  Pragmatics in Action: Indirect Requests Engage Theory of Mind Areas and the Cortical Motor Network , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  P. Vuilleumier,et al.  How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  P. McGuire,et al.  Engagement of right temporal cortex during processing of linguistic context , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  Ajay B. Satpute,et al.  Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain , 2013, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[40]  Brian A. Wandell,et al.  THE COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCES Fourth Edition , 2009 .

[41]  Brian Boyd,et al.  On the Origin of Stories: Evolution, Cognition, and Fiction , 2009 .

[42]  Penelope Brown,et al.  Politeness: Some Universals in Language Usage , 1989 .

[43]  Alberto Zani,et al.  The role of left and right hemispheres in the comprehension of idiomatic language: an electrical neuroimaging study , 2009, BMC Neuroscience.

[44]  Hongbo Yu,et al.  Cognitive empathy modulates the processing of pragmatic constraints during sentence comprehension. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[45]  Colin M. Brown,et al.  Early referential context effects in sentence processing: Evidence from event-related brain potentials , 1999 .

[46]  S. Levinson On the Human "Interaction Engine" , 2020, Roots of Human Sociality.

[47]  K. Davis,et al.  Two systems of resting state connectivity between the insula and cingulate cortex , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[48]  G. Fink,et al.  Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the “default system” of the brain , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[49]  Peter Hagoort,et al.  MUC (Memory, Unification, Control) and beyond , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[50]  Keith Oatley,et al.  The Function of Fiction is the Abstraction and Simulation of Social Experience , 2008, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[51]  Cara L. Wong,et al.  The function of the anterior temporal lobe: A review of the empirical evidence , 2012, Brain Research.

[52]  Ivan Toni,et al.  On the relationship between the “default mode network” and the “social brain” , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[53]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  The role of the right hemisphere in the interpretation of figurative aspects of language. A positron emission tomography activation study. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[54]  付伶俐 打磨Using Language,倡导新理念 , 2014 .

[55]  Siobhan Chapman Logic and Conversation , 2005 .

[56]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Situation models in language comprehension and memory. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.

[57]  Phillip J. Holcomb,et al.  Making sense of discourse: An fMRI study of causal inferencing across sentences , 2006, NeuroImage.

[58]  J. Decety,et al.  Human Empathy Through the Lens of Social Neuroscience , 2006, TheScientificWorldJournal.

[59]  Santoi Leung Language as Social Action: Social Psychology and Language Use , 2002 .

[60]  M. Kutas,et al.  Semantic integration in reading: engagement of the right hemisphere during discourse processing. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[61]  Evelyn C. Ferstl,et al.  The extended language network: A meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies on text comprehension , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[62]  Diana Van Lancker Sidtis,et al.  Where in the brain is nonliteral language , 2006 .

[63]  Matthew A. Lambon Ralph,et al.  Differential Contributions of Bilateral Ventral Anterior Temporal Lobe and Left Anterior Superior Temporal Gyrus to Semantic Processes , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[64]  P. Hagoort On Broca, brain, and binding: a new framework , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[65]  S. Pinker,et al.  Rationales for indirect speech: the theory of the strategic speaker. , 2010, Psychological review.

[66]  Rick W. Busselle,et al.  Fictionality and Perceived Realism in Experiencing Stories: A Model of Narrative Comprehension and Engagement , 2008 .

[67]  Michael Erb,et al.  Where in the brain is nonliteral language? A coordinate-based meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies , 2012, NeuroImage.

[68]  Mante S. Nieuwland,et al.  The Neurocognition of Referential Ambiguity in Language Comprehension , 2008, Lang. Linguistics Compass.

[69]  E. Goffman The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life , 1959 .

[70]  J. Perner,et al.  Common brain areas engaged in false belief reasoning and visual perspective taking: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[71]  T. Holtgraves,et al.  The Oxford handbook of language and social psychology , 2014 .

[72]  M. Slater,et al.  Entertainment—Education and Elaboration Likelihood: Understanding the Processing of Narrative Persuasion , 2002 .

[73]  T. Brock,et al.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology the Role of Transportation in the Persuasiveness of Public Narratives Text Quality Individual Differences and Situational Influences Transportation Scale Items Gender Differences Discriminant Validation: Need for Cognition Effect of Text Manipulation Beli , 2022 .

[74]  Evelina Fedorenko The role of domain-general cognitive control in language comprehension , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[75]  Jean-Luc Anton,et al.  Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox , 2010 .

[76]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Understanding by addressees and overhearers , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[77]  Van Berkum,et al.  The brain is a prediction machine that cares about good and bad - Any implications for neuropragmatics? , 2010 .

[78]  Jonathan Cohen Defining Identification: A Theoretical Look at the Identification of Audiences With Media Characters , 2001 .